Aging movement control

Normal aging movement control in humans is about the changes on the muscles, motor neurons, nerves, sensory functions, gait, fatigue, visual and manual responses, in men and women as they get older but who do not have neurological, muscular (atrophy, dystrophy...) or neuromuscular disorder. With aging, neuromuscular movements are impaired though with training or practice, some aspects may be prevented.

Force production

For voluntary force production, action potentials occur in the cortex. They propagate in the spinal cord, the motor neurons and the set of muscle fibers they innervate. This results in a twitch which properties are driven by two mechanisms: motor unit recruitment and rate coding. Both mechanisms are affected with aging. For instance, the number of motor units may decrease, the size of the motor units, i.e. the number of muscle fibers they innervate may increase, the frequency at which the action potentials are triggered may be reduced. Consequently, force production is generally impaired in old adults.[1]

Aging is associated with decreases in muscle mass and strength. These decreases may be partially due to losses of alpha motor neurons. By the age of 70, these losses occur in both proximal and distal muscles. In biceps brachii and brachialis, old adults show decreased strength (by 1/3) correlated with a reduction in the number of motor units (by 1/2). Old adults show evidence that remaining motor units may become larger as motor units innervate collateral muscle fibers.[2]

In first dorsal interosseus, almost all motor units are recruited at moderate rate coding, leading to 30-40% of maximal voluntary contraction (MVC). Motor unit discharge rates measured at 50% MVC are not significantly different in the young subjects from those observed in the old adults. However, for the maximal effort contractions, there is an appreciable difference in discharge rates between the two age groups. Discharge rates obtained at 100% of MVC are 64% smaller in the old adults than in the young subjects: 31.1 ± 11.8 impulses/s in the old subjects, 50.9 ± 19.5 impulses/s in the young subjects.[3]

Isometric strength and physical cross-sectional area of the elbow flexors and elbow extensors are reduced in old compared with young men. The normalized force (maximal voluntary force to the size of the muscle producing the force) of the elbow extensors is the same for old and young people. The normalized force for the elbow flexors is reduced in the old men compared to the young men. The lower normalized force of the elbow flexors may be due to an increase in agonist-antagonist muscles coactivation.[4]

Compared to the young group, the old group has lower dorsiflexors isometric torque at all angles, has lower knee extensors isometric torque at angles >90°. The impairment in force production is muscle specific. During dynamic exercise, the old group requires more time to reach a target velocity and is less able to attain high velocities. The slowing of voluntary contractile speed with age seems to play a role in the loss of dynamic torque.[5]

Sensory function

The detection of a stimulus by a receptor in the afferent nerve terminals (vs efferent nerve terminals) is useful to protect the body against unexpected disturbances. Studies in post-mortem subjects support that the thickness of muscle spindle capsules increases with age. There is a slight decrease in the number of intrafusal fibers in the oldest subjects. Some spindles show changes consistent with denervation associated with grouped denervation atrophy. Age-related changes are observed in fine structure of spindle nerve innervation in the form of axonal swelling and expanded/abnormal endplates.[6]

When subject to a task of proprioception, the elderly show increased cocontraction of agonist-antagonist muscles, perhaps to increase gamma drive and spindle sensitivity. It is believed to be used for postural control. Despite a cocontraction strategy, old adults have higher reaction time and also make greater errors in estimating the position of their ankle. The elderly subjects with greater errors for the dynamic position sense also perform poorly on the single limb stance eyes closed test.[7]

Old adults sway more than young adults while maintaining upright standing posture, especially with eyes closed with a narrow base of support. Young adults show “resourcefulness” by shifting from one sensory input (vision) to another (somatosensory) whereas old adults do not rely on the variety of sensory inputs but rather respond by stiffening their ankles across tasks (wide base of support vs narrow base of support, eyes open vs eyes closed).[8]

Sensory receptors can initiate rapid responses to perturbations thanks to short-latency connections between afferent innervations and motor units. Yet, aging results in decreases in motor conduction velocities. This may be due to losses of the fastest conducting motor units. There is also evidence of slowing of both fast and slow conducting axons which can be explained by decreases in axon diameter through demyelination, by reduction of internodal length. Some studies suggest an overall decrease in the number of myelinated fibers.[9]

Aging results in slowed reaction time in an aiming task for both eye and hand movements. Comparisons between young and old adults who have to follow a target only with their eyes or with a laser in their hand, show that parameters indicative of motor function such as velocity, duration, and amplitude of initial movement are unchanged. However the duration of corrective movement is longer for old adults. It suggests an impairment to sensory system.[10]

Walking gait

When confronted to an unexpected slip or trip during walking, compared to young adults, old adults have a less effective balance strategy: smaller and slower postural muscle responses, altered temporal and spatial organization of the postural response, agonist-antagonist muscles coactivation and greater upper trunk instability. Comparing control and slip conditions, after the perturbation, young adults have a longer stride length, a longer stride duration, and the same walk velocity whereas old adults have a shorter stride length, the same stride duration, and a lower walk velocity.[11]

In an experiment, for a single-task walking, 24% of old adults have gait speed <0.8 m/s but for a dual-task of walking and talking, 62% of old adults have gait speed <0.8 m/s. In practical terms, this means that a large proportion of healthy community-dwelling old adults may not walk fast enough to safely cross the street while simultaneously having a conversation. These findings support the assertion that generating spontaneous speech is highly demanding on cognitive resources and suggest that real world dual-task effects on gait may be underestimated by reaction time tasks.[12]

Fatigue resistance

Compared to young adults, old adults exhibit muscle fatigue (peripheral fatigue) resistance during sustained isometric maximal voluntary contraction, but they show greater supraspinal fatigue at start of sustained task, and during recovery. The first observation reflects changes in fiber type ratio; with aging the proportion of type I muscle fibers which are adapted to long effort becomes greater. The second observation is likely a result of cumulative effects of exercise on the central nervous system.[13]

For the knee extensors, old adults produce less torque during dynamic or isometric maximal voluntary contractions than young adults. The mechanisms controlling fatigue in the elderly during isometric contractions are not the same as those that influence fatigue during dynamic contractions, while young adults keep the same strategy. The knee extensors of healthy old adults fatigue less during isometric contractions than do those of young adults who had similar levels of habitual physical activity. In contrast, there are no differences between age groups in the fatigue during dynamic contractions.[14]

Speed, dexterity

For old adults, the decreased saccadic accuracy, prolonged latency, and reduced saccadic velocity may be explained by cerebral cortical degeneration with age. Old adults show reduced amplitude of primary saccades and they generally more saccades to reach fixation. Old adults show significant delay of saccades in all conditions (predictable amplitude and time target steps, unpredictable amplitude target steps, unpredictabe time target steps). Age-related slowing is only evident for predictable targets; however other studies have shown otherwise, but noted higher variance in speed of old adults.[15]

Instructed to look either toward (pro-saccade task) or away from (anti-saccade task) an eccentric target under different conditions of fixation, for young children (5±8 years of age) a long time elapses between the apparition of the target and the onset of the eye movement (Saccadic Reaction Time). Young adults (20±30 years of age) typically have the fastest SRTs. Elderly subjects (60±79 years of age) have slower SRTs and longer duration saccades than any other age groups.[16]

Old adults exhibit reductions in manual dexterity which is observed through changes in fingertip force when gripping and/or lifting. Compared to young adults, old adults show an increase in grip force and safety margins (minimum force necessary to prevent a slip). These increases can be explained by skin slipperiness or it may be result of declining cutaneous information. Force increases are not associated with impaired capacity to modulate fingertip forces smoothly. There is no evidence that old adults were less able to program fingertips based on the memory of a preceding lift.[17]

The prismatic grasp (4 fingers in opposition to thumb) which is common in everyday activities, involves the organization of the digits into specific tasks and the balance of force/moment production by individual digits. Old adults exhibit an impairment in finger and hand force production. They show excessive grip force which could be related to higher moments produced by antagonist fingers. Both can be viewed as energetically suboptimal but more stable performance.[18]

Old adults often show heightened antagonist muscle coactivation during goal directed movement. Contractions at moderate-to-high force often show activation of other ipsilateral and contralateral muscles. When the itensity of contralateral activity is sufficient to produce movement, this is called “mirror movement”. When asked to follow a unilateral task, young and old adults show concurrent activity in contralateral muscle but it is greater in old adults. Contralateral activity is greater for isometric than for anisometric contractions. Contralateral force is greater for eccentric than concentric contractions.[19]

Training consequences

Type I muscle fiber characteristics (area, number of capillary contacts, fiber area/capillary contacts) of the vastus lateralis are unaffected by age. The old men normal fit or trained have smaller type II muscle fiber areas and fewer capillaries surrounding these fibers than do the young men. The capillary supply per unit type II fiber area is not affected by age but is enhanced by training. The old trained men have succinate dehydrogenase activities within their type IIa muscle fibers similar to those in young men and twofold higher than in old normal fit men.[20]

Neural changes like reduced motor unit discharge rates, increased variability of motor unit discharge activity, altered recruitment and derecruitment behavior mediate modifications in muscle control. On the other hand, physiological deleterious factors including motor unit loss, increased motor unit innervation ratios also affect muscle force. Through strength training, old adults can significantly improve their force control. The rapid adaptation suggests modifications in motor unit activation, increased excitability of motoneuron pool, and decreased antagonist cocontraction.[21]

Heavy resistance and sensorimotor trainings result in increased maximum voluntary contraction and rate force development. But sensorimotor training shows more positive adaptations in postural reflexes, which is likely due to training of sensory reception/processing, central integration of afferent information, transformation of that information into adequate efferent response. The decreased onset latency and increased magnitude of reflex response with sensorimotor training is associated with increased ankle joint stiffness during perturbations.[22]

When asked to reach a given level of force at a certain moment in time without any visual feedback, old adults are less accurate than young adults. With the practice of goal-directed contractions, old adults can improve the accuracy of novel motor tasks (isometric or dynamic) though their strategy differs from the strategy used by young adults. For both age groups, the greatest improvements in accuracy occur at the beginning of practice.[23]

Old adults are able to improve the modulation of grasping forces after motor practice. Unexpectedly, motor practice fails to reduce grasping performance losses under the dual-task conditions but motor practice reduces the decline in cognitive performance under dual-task conditions. Therefore, motor practice seems to free up cognitive resources that were previously monitoring motor performance and old adults seemed to use these resources to improve their cognitive performance under dual-task conditions.[24]

References

  1. Neuromechanics of human movement, chapter 6 Single-joint system function; Roger M. Enoka.
  2. Doherty TJ, Vandervoort AA, Taylor AW, Brown WF (February 1993). "Effects of motor unit losses on strength in older men and women". J. Appl. Physiol. 74 (2): 868–74. PMID 8458808.
  3. Kamen G, Sison SV, Du CC, Patten C (December 1995). "Motor unit discharge behavior in older adults during maximal-effort contractions". J. Appl. Physiol. 79 (6): 1908–13. PMID 8847252.
  4. Klein CS, Rice CL, Marsh GD (September 2001). "Normalized force, activation, and coactivation in the arm muscles of young and old men". J. Appl. Physiol. 91 (3): 1341–9. PMID 11509534.
  5. Lanza IR, Towse TF, Caldwell GE, Wigmore DM, Kent-Braun JA (December 2003). "Effects of age on human muscle torque, velocity, and power in two muscle groups". J. Appl. Physiol. 95 (6): 2361–9. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00724.2002. PMID 12923120.
  6. Swash M, Fox KP (August 1972). "The effect of age on human skeletal muscle. Studies of the morphology and innervation of muscle spindles". J. Neurol. Sci. 16 (4): 417–32. doi:10.1016/0022-510X(72)90048-2. PMID 4261815.
  7. Madhavan S, Shields RK (July 2005). "Influence of age on dynamic position sense: evidence using a sequential movement task". Exp Brain Res 164 (1): 18–28. doi:10.1007/s00221-004-2208-3. PMID 15776224.
  8. Benjuya N, Melzer I, Kaplanski J (February 2004). "Aging-induced shifts from a reliance on sensory input to muscle cocontraction during balanced standing". J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 59 (2): 166–71. doi:10.1093/gerona/59.2.M166. PMID 14999032.
  9. Wang FC, de Pasqua V, Delwaide PJ (August 1999). "Age-related changes in fastest and slowest conducting axons of thenar motor units". Muscle Nerve 22 (8): 1022–9. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-4598(199908)22:8<1022::AID-MUS3>3.0.CO;2-F. PMID 10417782.
  10. Warabi T, Noda H, Kato T (June 1986). "Effect of aging on sensorimotor functions of eye and hand movements". Exp. Neurol. 92 (3): 686–97. doi:10.1016/0014-4886(86)90309-2. PMID 3709742.
  11. Tang, PF.; Woollacott, MH. (Nov 1998). "Inefficient postural responses to unexpected slips during walking in older adults.". J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 53 (6): M471–80. doi:10.1093/gerona/53A.6.M471. PMID 9823752.
  12. Plummer-D'Amato, P.; Altmann, LJ.; Reilly, K. (Feb 2011). "Dual-task effects of spontaneous speech and executive function on gait in aging: exaggerated effects in slow walkers.". Gait Posture 33 (2): 233–7. doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.11.011. PMID 21193313.
  13. Hunter, SK.; Todd, G.; Butler, JE.; Gandevia, SC.; Taylor, JL. (Oct 2008). "Recovery from supraspinal fatigue is slowed in old adults after fatiguing maximal isometric contractions.". J Appl Physiol 105 (4): 1199–209. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.01246.2007. PMID 18687979.
  14. Callahan, DM.; Foulis, SA.; Kent-Braun, JA. (May 2009). "Age-related fatigue resistance in the knee extensor muscles is specific to contraction mode.". Muscle Nerve 39 (5): 692–702. doi:10.1002/mus.21278. PMC: 2718567. PMID 19347926.
  15. Sharpe, JA.; Zackon, DH. (1987). "Senescent saccades. Effects of aging on their accuracy, latency and velocity.". Acta Otolaryngol 104 (5-6): 422–8. doi:10.3109/00016488709128270. PMID 3434263.
  16. Munoz, DP.; Broughton, JR.; Goldring, JE.; Armstrong, IT. (Aug 1998). "Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks.". Exp Brain Res 121 (4): 391–400. doi:10.1007/s002210050473. PMID 9746145.
  17. Cole, KJ.; Rotella, DL.; Harper, JG. (Apr 1999). "Mechanisms for age-related changes of fingertip forces during precision gripping and lifting in adults.". J Neurosci 19 (8): 3238–47. PMID 10191336.
  18. Shim, JK.; Lay, BS.; Zatsiorsky, VM.; Latash, ML. (Jul 2004). "Age-related changes in finger coordination in static prehension tasks.". J Appl Physiol 97 (1): 213–24. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00045.2004. PMC: 2832863. PMID 15003998.
  19. Shinohara, M.; Keenan, KG.; Enoka, RM. (Mar 2003). "Contralateral activity in a homologous hand muscle during voluntary contractions is greater in old adults.". J Appl Physiol 94 (3): 966–74. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00836.2002. PMID 12433847.
  20. Proctor, DN.; Sinning, WE.; Walro, JM.; Sieck, GC.; Lemon, PW. (Jun 1995). "Oxidative capacity of human muscle fiber types: effects of age and training status.". J Appl Physiol 78 (6): 2033–8. PMID 7665396.
  21. Patten, C.; Kamen, G. (Oct 2000). "Adaptations in motor unit discharge activity with force control training in young and older human adults.". Eur J Appl Physiol 83 (2-3): 128–43. doi:10.1007/s004210000271. PMID 11104053.
  22. Granacher, U.; Gollhofer, A.; Strass, D. (Dec 2006). "Training induced adaptations in characteristics of postural reflexes in elderly men.". Gait Posture 24 (4): 459–66. doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.12.007. PMID 16472525.
  23. Christou, EA.; Poston, B.; Enoka, JA.; Enoka, RM. (May 2007). "Different neural adjustments improve endpoint accuracy with practice in young and old adults.". J Neurophysiol 97 (5): 3340–50. doi:10.1152/jn.01138.2006. PMID 17376846.
  24. Voelcker-Rehage, C.; Alberts, JL. (May 2007). "Effect of motor practice on dual-task performance in older adults.". J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 62 (3): P141–8. doi:10.1093/geronb/62.3.p141. PMID 17507581.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Saturday, April 16, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.