Blood type personality theory
Type A | |
---|---|
Best traits | Earnest, sensible, reserved, patient, responsible |
Worst traits | Fastidious, overearnest, stubborn, tense |
Type B | |
Best traits | Passionate, active, doer, creative, strong |
Worst traits | irresponsible, unforgiving, "going own way" |
Type AB | |
Best traits | Cool, controlled, rational, sociable, adaptable |
Worst traits | Critical, indecisive, forgetful, irresponsible, "split personality" |
Type O | |
Best traits | Confident, self-determined, optimistic, strong-willed, intuitive |
Worst traits | Self-centered, cold, doubtful, unpredictable, "workaholic" |
In Japan and other East Asian countries, a person's ABO blood type or ketsueki-gata (血液型) is believed by many to be predictive of personality, temperament, and compatibility with others.[1] This is similar to how astrological signs are perceived as influencing factors in a person's life in other countries.
One of the reasons Japan developed the blood type theory was in reaction against ethnic stereotypes coming from Europe.[2] The popular belief originates with publications by Masahiko Nomi in the 1970s. The scientific community generally dismisses such beliefs as superstition or pseudoscience because of the lack demonstrable evidence or reference to testable criteria.[1][3][4] Although research into the causal link between blood type and personality is limited, research conclusively demonstrates no statistically significant association.[5][6][7][8] However, some studies suggest statistically significant relationships, although as self-fulfilling phenomena of persons acting in accordance with their assumed blood type personality rather than their blood type biologically influencing their personality.[9][10][11] Recently, some medical hypotheses have been proposed.[12][13]
History
The ABO blood group system is widely credited to the Austrian scientist Karl Landsteiner, who discovered three blood types in 1900.[14]
In 1926, Rin Hirano and Tomita Yashima published the article "Blood Type Biological Related" in the Army Medical Journal. It was seen to be a non-statistical and unscientific report, motivated by racism.
Takeji Furukawa
In 1927, Takeji Furukawa, a professor at Tokyo Women's Teacher's School, published his paper "The Study of Temperament Through Blood Type" in the scholarly journal Psychological Research. The idea quickly took off with the Japanese public despite Furukawa's lack of credentials, and the militarist government of the time commissioned a study aimed at breeding ideal soldiers.[1] The study used ten to twenty people for the investigation, thereby failing to meet the statistical requirements for generalizing the results to the wider population.
On the other hand, in 1934, Fisher announced the chi-square test, which is very popular at present, for the first time. Several scholars said that they found statistically significant differences analyzing Japanese works conducted at that time.[15]
In another study, Furukawa compared the distribution of blood types among two ethnic groups: the Formosans in Taiwan and the Ainu of Hokkaidō. His motivation for the study appears to have come from a political incident:[16] After the Japanese occupation of Taiwan following Japan's invasion of China in 1895, the inhabitants tenaciously resisted their occupiers. Insurgencies in 1930 and in 1931 resulted in the deaths of hundreds of Japanese settlers.[16]
The purpose of Furukawa's studies was to "penetrate the essence of the racial traits of the Taiwanese, who recently revolted and behaved so cruelly". Based on a finding that 41.2% of a Taiwanese sample had type O blood, Furukawa assumed that the Taiwanese rebelliousness was genetically determined. The reasoning was supported by the fact that among the Ainu, whose temperament was characterized as submissive, only 23.8% had type O. In conclusion, Furukawa suggested that the Japanese should increase intermarriage with the Taiwanese to reduce the number of Taiwanese with type O blood.[16]
Masahiko Nomi
Interest in the theory faded in the 1930s. It was revived in the 1970s with a book by Masahiko Nomi, a journalist, with no medical background (he graduated from the engineering faculty of University of Tokyo). Few Japanese psychologists criticized him at that time,[17] so he continued to demonstrate statistically significant data in various fields and published several books with these results.[18] Quite some time after his death in 1981, Masahiko Nomi's work was said to be largely uncontrolled and anecdotal, and the methodology of his conclusions was unclear.[19] Because of this, he was heavily criticized by the Japanese psychological community, although his books remain popular.[19] His son Toshitaka Nomi continued to promote the theory with a series of books and by running the Institute of Blood Type Humanics.[20] He later established the Human Science ABO Center for further research and publication in 2004.
Background and criticism
Criticism
Some researchers believe blood types are associated with personality traits. Many medical surveys have been conducted, such as those for hormone secretion traits (DBH, COMT, MAO etc.),[21] or the blood-type antigens in neurons, or the antigens expressed on surfaces of brain cells. But these studies are not yet fully confirmed, except that the ABO antigens are expressed in early embryos.[22]
There are also many psychological or statistical approaches, but these are not yet completely confirmed, either. Quite a few academic researchers consider Furukawa and Nomi's studies as neither testified nor correct, because they used neither statistically appropriate measures nor methods and so had not shown sufficient evidence, i.e. random sampling, correct usage of statistical tests.
For example, Kengo Nawata, a Japanese social psychologist, statistically analyzed three data sets of over 10,000 Japanese and American people in total.[8] However, 65 of the 68 items yielded non-significant differences between blood types and the other three items showed relatively slight relationships. Therefore, the blood type explained only 0.3% of the whole differences of these data sets. This result suggests that blood type explained very little of people's personalities. Nawata came to the conclusion that there is actually no relevance of blood type for personality.
Controversial statistically significant data
However, some academic researchers have shown several statistically significant data in Japan and Korea. Akira Sakamoto and Kenji Yamazaki, Japanese social psychologists, analyzed 32,347 samples of annual opinion polls from 1978 through 1988.[9][10] These results indicated that Japanese blood-typical stereotypes actually influenced their self-reported personalities—as a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Cosy Muto and Masahiro Nagashima et al. (Nagasaki University) conducted a supplementary survey of Yamazaki and Sakamoto in 2011.[11] They demonstrated that significant and the same difference on personalities between blood-types by using the same database as Samamoto and Yamazaki used. In the 1990s, difference due to blood types was stabilized and variances became smaller. Then in the 2000s, difference was definitely statistically significant, too. However, the size of the difference was extremely small so that not "usable" in everyday life.
Another Japanese social psychologist, Shigeyuki Yamaoka (Shotoku University), announced results of his questionnaires, which were conducted in 1999 (1,300 subjects)[23] and 2006 (1,362 subjects),[24] In both cases, the subjects were university students, and only subjects with enough knowledge of and belief in the "blood-type diagnosis" showed meaningful differences. He concluded that these differences must be the influence of mass media, especially TV programs. Yamaoka later examined 6,660 samples from 1999 through 2009 in total and found the same result.[25]
On the other hand, there are opinions that the statistically meaningful differences according to the blood types are not explained only by beliefs, nor are they a self-fulfilling prophecy. In Japan, penetration rate of blood-typical personality traits were investigated. Yoriko Watanabe, a Japanese psychologist (then Hokkaido University), chose "well-known" traits and found most traits were known to no more than half of Japanese (subjects were university students).[26] A Japanese writer, Masayuki Kanazawa, analyzed these blood-typical traits in combination with data from Yamaoka (1999)[23] that used the same items of Watanabe's penetration survey.[26] If blood-typical differences are caused by penetration (or their self-recognition), the rate of difference of a trait is proportional to the rate of its penetration. However, Kanazawa was not able to discover any association with blood-type differences and penetration rates.[27] This result raises doubt about the role of beliefs and self-fulfilling prophecy.
Most reports that demonstrated statistical correlation attribute differences to self-fulfilling prophecy. However, there is no study that directly proved the existence of "self fulfillment". Therefore, opinions of researchers are varied at present: 1. whether there is statistical correlation or not, 2. whether any statistical correlations are superficial, being caused by subjects' self-fulfilling prophecy, or if they are truly caused by the blood type.
In addition, a very strange phenomenon is observed. A certain personality test will not detect differences of blood type. The famous "big five" personality test was carried out in several countries, including Japan, Korea, and Taiwan, after the year 2000. No researchers found meaningful statistical difference.[5][6][7][28][29] The big five personality test is intended to digitize traits of self-rating or self-recognition. Therefore, it was expected that differences in self-reported personalities (as self-prophecy) would be detected from the subject who believed blood-typical stereotypes. The result is, as mentioned above, that no meaningful statistical difference was found until now. So Ho Cho, a Korean psychologist (Yonsei University), and the others carried out a questionnaire about blood-typical items to subjects (all university students) and discovered statistical differences as expected.[28] However, the difference was not found when the big five personality test was administered to the same subjects. Another Korean researcher Sohn (Yonsei University) re-analyzed Cho's data.[30] He found that several independent items of the big five personality test detected differences according to each blood-typical stereotype. However, these differences became extinct in the process of plural items being gathered to five factors (big five).
If these results are correct, the big five personality test cannot detect differences between the blood types, if such a causational link did indeed exist. [9][10][23][24][25] Interestingly, the phenomenon is not reported such as MBTI, except the big five test.
Brain waves and light topography
Kim and Yi (Seoul University of Venture & Information) measured brain waves of 4,636 adults. They reported that type O people were most stress-resistant.[31] In addition, an experiment using light topography instruments by Munetaka Haida (Tokai University School of Medicine) suggests the possibility that activated parts of human brain are different according to blood types. i.e. type A's left brain is superior to the right, while type B's right brain is superior.[32]
In this way, it is seen that no agreeable conclusion exists at present, although many statistical surveys, reexaminations, supplementary investigations have been carried out by many researchers.
- Opinions of researchers
Opinions | Blood Type and Personality | Consistency with Statistical Data | Consistency with Brain Wave Research |
---|---|---|---|
No statistical |
No relationship |
[OK] Can explain statistically non-significant data |
[NG] Cannot explain statistically significant data |
Statistically |
No relationship |
[OK] Can explain statistically significant data |
[NG] Cannot explain statistically significant data |
Statistically |
Real relationship |
[OK] Can explain statistically significant data |
[OK] Can explain statistically significant data |
Current popularity
Discussion of blood types is widely popular in women's magazines as a way of gauging relationship compatibility with a potential or current partner. Morning television shows feature blood type horoscopes, and similar horoscopes are published daily in newspapers. The blood types of celebrities are listed in their infoboxes on Japanese Wikipedia.[33] A series of four books that describe people's character by blood type ranked third, fourth, fifth and ninth on a list of best-selling books in Japan in 2008 compiled by Tohan Corporation.[34]
No less than two-thirds people in several East Asian countries, such as Japan, Korea and Taiwan, believe in the association with blood types and personality.[35]
In addition, according to one Japanese survey, more than half of Japanese people are fond of talking about blood type and personality.[36] The research also says that people in Japan like blood-typical personality diagnosis and 1. talk about it with proper knowledge, 2. believe some relationships exist between blood type and personality, 3. feel its traits are applicable to themselves to a certain degree. Results of two other surveys are the same.[37]
Although there is no proven correlation between blood type and personality, it remains popular with the many matchmaking services that cater to blood type. In this way, it is similar to the use of astrological signs, which is also popular in Japan. Asking one's blood type is common in Japan, and people are often surprised when a non-Japanese does not know their own blood type.[38]
It is common among anime and manga authors to mention their character's blood types and to give their characters blood types to match their personalities.[39] Some video game characters have known blood types. In addition, it is common for video game series to allow for blood type as an option in their creation modes.[39]
Blood type harassment, called "bura-hara" (wasei-eigo-a portmanteau of "blood" and "harassment"), has been blamed for bullying of children in playgrounds, loss of job opportunities, and ending of happy relationships.[40]
Many people have been discriminated against because of their blood type. Employers ask blood types during interviews despite the warnings they have been given. Children at schools have been split up according to their blood type. The national softball team has customized training to fit each player's blood type. Companies have given work assignments according to their employee's blood type.[41]
Facebook in many Asian countries allows users to include their blood type in their profile.[42]
After then-Reconstruction Minister Ryu Matsumoto's abrasive comments towards the governors of Iwate and Miyagi[43] forced him to step down from his post, he partially blamed his behavior on his blood type, saying "My blood is type B, which means I can be irritable and impetuous, and my intentions don't always come across."[44]
But these episodes are thought to be more or less exaggerated—as well as horoscope addiction in the Europe or US, which is sometimes reported in Japan. No blood-type harassment trials have been reported, so far. In reality, most Japanese people don't think blood types determine their personalities, but affect it in some degree.[36][37]
Blood types are treated as important in South Korea as well. An example can be seen in the film My Boyfriend Is Type B where a girl is advised not to date a man because of his blood type. The Korean webcomic A Simple Thinking About Blood Type depicts the stereotypes of each blood type, and has been adapted as a short anime series in Japan as Ketsuekigata-kun! in 2013 and 2015.
Notes
- 1 2 3 Yamaguchi, Mari (6 May 2005). "Myth about Japan blood types under attack". MediResource Inc. Archived from the original on 28 December 2009. Retrieved 29 December 2007.
- ↑ Takeji Furukawa (1927), 血液型と気質 Blood Type and Temperament - in Europe As are more than Asia, so Europeans insisted A races are superior to B races. A Japanese scholar, Takeji Furukawa opposed the idea and asserted that B persons were active while A persons were passive.
- ↑ Dating by blood type in Japan
- ↑ Nuwer, Rachel. "You are what you bleed: In Japan and other east Asian countries some believe blood type dictates personality". Scientific American. Retrieved 16 Feb 2011.
- 1 2 Cramer, K. M., & Imaike, E. (2002). Personality, blood type, and the five-factor model. Personality and individual differences, 32(4), 621-626.
- 1 2 Rogers, M., & Glendon, A. I. (2003). Blood type and personality. Personality and individual differences, 34(7), 1099-1112.
- 1 2 3 Wu, K., Lindsted, K. D., & Lee, J. W. (2005). Blood type and the five factors of personality in Asia. Personality and individual differences, 38(4), 797-808.
- 1 2 Kengo Nawata (2014), No relationship between blood type and personality: Evidence from large-scale surveys in Japan and the US, The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 85(2), 148-156.
- 1 2 3 Sakamoto, A., & Yamazaki, K. (2004), Blood-typical personality stereotypes and self-fulfilling prophecy: A natural experiment with time-series data of 1978–1988., Progress in Asian Social Psychology, Vol. 4, 239–262.
- 1 2 3 Yamazaki, K., & Sakamoto, A. (1992), 血液型ステレオタイプによる自己成就現象II-全国調査の時系列分析- The self-fulfillment phenomenon generated by blood-typical personality stereotypes: time-series analysis of nation-wide survey II, Paper presented at the 33rd annual convention of the Japanese society of social psychology. Tokyo (pp. 342-345).
- 1 2 Cosy Muto, Masahiro Nagashima et. al. (2011), A Demonstrative and Critical Study on Pseudo-science for Scientific Literacy Construction at Teacher Education Course , FY2011 Final Research Report from the Database of Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research - neither exact number of samples nor years was specified in the report
- ↑ Donna K. Hobgood (2011), Personality traits of aggression-submissiveness and perfectionism associate with ABO blood groups through catecholamine activities, Medical Hypotheses, 77(2):294-300.
- ↑ Shoko Tsuchimine, Junji Saruwatari, Ayako Kaneda, Norio Yasui-Furukori (2015), ABO Blood Type and Personality Traits in Healthy Japanese Subjects
- ↑ Landsteiner, K. (1900). "Zur Kenntnis der antifermentativen, lytischen und agglutinierenden Wirkungen des Blutserums und der Lymphe". Zentralblatt Bakteriologie 27: 357–62.
- ↑ e.g. Masao Omura (Nihon University) in "血液型と性格 Blood Type and Personality", Terumitsu Maekawa (Asia University) in "血液型人間学 Blood Type Humanics" and so on.
- 1 2 3 Becker, Peter (Ed.); Yoji Nakatani (2006). "The Birth of Criminology in Modern Japan". Criminals and their Scientists: The History of Criminology in International Perspective (Publications of the German Historical Institute). Cambridge University Press. p. 294. ISBN 0-521-81012-4.
- ↑ Toshinori Shirasa & Takuji Iguchi (1993), 血液型性格研究入門 血液型と性格は関係ないと言えるのか An introduction to Blood Type Personality Research - Can we say there is no relationship with blood type and personality?, pp.209-212 & 242-243. -- virtually no papers nor books about blood type and personality was published from the Japanese psychological community before Masahiko Nomi's death in 1981.
- ↑ Masahiko Nomi analyzed various data by using statistical methods and found meaningful traits; the following are some of his works.
- Blood Type Affinity Study 5/1974 - 20,000 samples analyzed in total
- Blood Type Sports Study 10/1976 - 1,000 track-and-field athletes analyzed
- Blood Type Essence 6/1977 - listed over 1,000 people (politicians, CEOs, artists, etc.)
- Blood Type Politics Study 6/1978 - 2,000 politicians analyzed (all representatives of the national Diet, all governors, and all mayors)
- 1 2 D'Adamo, Dr. Peter J. (2002). The Eat Right for Your Type: Complete Blood Type Encyclopedia. Riverhead Trade. p. 28. ISBN 1-57322-920-2.
- ↑ Evans, Ruth (4 November 2012). "Japan and blood types: Does it determine personality?". BBC News. Retrieved 5 November 2012.
- ↑ Donna K. Hobgood (2011) Personality traits of aggression-submissiveness and perfectionism associate with ABO blood groups through catecholamine activities. Medical Hypotheses, 77(2), 294-300.
- ↑ Szulman, A. E. (1980). The ABH Blood Groups and development. Current Topics in Developmental Biology, 14, 127-145.
- 1 2 3 Shigeyuki Yamaoka (1999), 血液型ステレオタイプが生み出す血液型差別の研究 A Study on Blood Harassment Caused by Blood-typical Stereotypes, Paper presented at the 40th annual convention of the Japanese society of social psychology. Tokyo. - for further information of this paper, confer to Shigeyuki Yamaoka (2001), ダメな大人にならないための心理学 A Psychology Book for not to Become a Useless Adult, pp.35-73 ISBN 4892426652
- 1 2 Shigeyuki Yamaoka (2006), 血液型性格項目の自己認知に及ぼすTV番組視聴の影響 Influence of Watching TV programs to One's Self-recognition of the Blood-type personality Items Paper presented at the 47th annual convention of the Japanese society of social psychology. Tokyo.
- 1 2 Shigeyuki Yamaoka (2009), 血液型性格判断の差別性と虚妄性(自主企画(2)) Segregation and falsehood of blood-type personality analysis, self planning session (2) at the 18th annual convention of the Japanese society of personality psychology.
- 1 2 Watanabe, Y. (1994). 血液型ステレオタイプ形成におけるプロトタイプとイグゼンブラの役割 The roles of prototype and exemplar in the formation of the "blood type stereotype". Japanese Journal of Social Psychology, 10-2, 77-86. - She extracted 7 traits for each 4 blood types, 28 in total, which were common to three or more "blood type diagnosis" books. 20 items of all 28 showed less than 50% penetration (the average is 46.1%).
- ↑ Masayuki Kanazawa (2014), 統計でわかる血液型人間学入門 An Introduction to Blood Type Humanics - Understanding by Statistics, Gentosha Runaissance ISBN 4779011094/9784779011092 pp.16-36
- 1 2 So Hyun Cho, Eun Kook M. Suh, Yoen Jung Ro (2005), Beliefs about Blood Types and Traits and Their Reflections in Self-reported Personality, Korean Journal of Social and Personality Psychology, 19(4), 37-47.
- ↑ Yoshio Kubo, Yukiko Miyake (2011), 血液型と性格の関連についての調査的研究 Correlation between blood types and personalities Bulletin of Kibi International University (Department of Social Welfare), 21, 93-100.
- ↑ Sung Il Ryu , Young Woo Sohn (2007), A Review of Sociocultural, Behavioral, Biochemical Analyses on ABO Blood-Groups Typology, The Korean Journal of Social and Personality Psychology
- ↑ Choong-Shik Kim, Seon-Gyu Yi (2011), A Study on the effects of one's blood type on emotional character and antistress of adults, Journal of the Korea Academia-Industrial cooperation Society, 12(6), 2554-2560. - According to this article "meaningful difference had been revealed between the blood type and stress resistancy; type O rated higher scores in awareness and stress resistancy then other types..."
- ↑ Human Science ABO Center held the symposium "血液型を考える~ヒトABO式血液型遺伝子を理解する為に~ Think about Blood Type -- Toward Understanding Human ABO Genes" in February, 2010 (Tokyo). -- the report (Japanese) including Haida's presentation is here.
- ↑ "Type Cast: The Japanese Fascination with Blood Types". Retrieved 21 July 2012.
- ↑ Blood Types -- Do They Shape a Personality or Mere Stereotypes, Natsuko Fukue, The Japan Times, December 31, 2008
- ↑ Many people of the East Asia believe the association with blood types and personality; Japan: 75% (NHK - Japan Broadcasting Corporation - opinion polls, 1986), Korea: 75% (Wow Korea, July 21, 2014), Taiwan: 66% (Wu et. al[7])
- 1 2 Reiko Yamashita (2008), 血液型性格判断はなぜすたれないのか why doesn't blood type in Japanense culture detariorate?, Paper presented at the 48th annual convention of the Japanese society of social psychology.
- 1 2 The followings are results of two Japanese surveys:
- Yahoo! Research (December 25, 2008)『「血液型本」に関する調査』 A survey about "blood type books" -- "My personality is appropriately expressed so that sympathized, convinced" (66%), "Can grasp my personality objectively with new discovery" (59%), "One of the means and the methods to express my personality" (50%), "Useful in acquaintances" (48%) and "Useful in love-affiliated things" (47%).
- Yumiko Kamise, Yutaka Matsui (1996), 血液型ステレオタイプ変容の形 ―ステレオタイプ変容モデルの検証― Changing processes of stereotype on blood-groups, Japanese Journal of Social Psychology, 11-3, 170-179. -- "blood-typical personality diagnosis is a fun" (83.6%), "I like blood-typical personality diagnosis" (61.5%).
- ↑ In Japan, you are what your blood type is, Japan Today
- 1 2 Brenner, Robin E. (2007). Understanding manga and anime. Libraries Unlimited. p. 41. ISBN 978-1-59158-332-5.
- ↑ McCurry, Justin (4 December 2008). "Typecast - Japan's obsession with blood groups". The Guardian. Retrieved 14 December 2012.
- ↑ Yamaguchi, Mari (2009-02-01). "In Japan, Your Blood Type Says It All". Huffington Post. Retrieved 2014-04-12.
- ↑ Blood Type Personality , Psychologia, Retrieved 17 May 2014.
- ↑ Fukue, Natsuko (2011-07-05). "Matsumoto rips Tohoku governors". The Japan Times. Retrieved 2014-04-12.
- ↑ Lies, Elaine (2011-07-06). "Blame it on my blood, disgraced Japan politician says". Reuters.
Dr LAURENT: "4groupes sanguins, 4 personnalités" Marco Pietteur ed. 2007/2014. Belgium. (In French)
Further reading
- Toshitaka Nomi and Alexander Besher, You Are Your Blood Type: the biochemical key to unlocking the secrets of your personality. New York: Pocket Books, 1988. ISBN 0-671-63342-2
- Peter Constantine What's Your Type?: How Blood Types are the Keys to Unlocking Your Personality. 1997. Plume, ISBN 0452278023
- Laura Miller, People Types: Personality Classification in Japanese Women's Magazines, The Journal of Popular Culture, Vol. 11, No. 2, Fall 1977, pp. 436–452.
- Sakamoto, A., & Yamazaki, K. (2004), Blood-typical personality stereotypes and self-fulfilling prophecy: A natural experiment with time-series data of 1978–1988., Progress in Asian Social Psychology, Vol. 4, 239–262.
- Beom Jun Kim et al. (2007), Blood-type Distribution, Physica A: Statistical and Theoretical Physics 373(1), 533-540.
- Kengo Nawata (2014), No relationship between blood type and personality: Evidence from large-scale surveys in Japan and the US, The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 85(2), 148-156.
External links
- Blood type and the five factors of personality in Asia
- Japanese Blood Types
- Human Science ABO Center
- Website of Dr. Peter J. D'Adamo