Cognitive load
In cognitive psychology, cognitive load refers to the total amount of mental effort being used in the working memory. Cognitive load theory was developed out of the study of problem solving by John Sweller in the late 1980s.[1] Sweller argued that instructional design can be used to reduce cognitive load in learners. Cognitive load theory differentiates cognitive load into three types: intrinsic, extraneous, and germane.
Intrinsic cognitive load is the effort associated with a specific topic. Extraneous cognitive load refers to the way information or tasks are presented to a learner. And, germane cognitive load refers to the work put into creating a permanent store of knowledge, or a schema.
Researchers Paas and Van Merriënboer developed a way to measure perceived mental effort which is indicative of cognitive load.[2] Task-invoked pupillary response is a reliable and sensitive measurement of cognitive load that is directly related to working memory.[3] Heavy cognitive load can have negative effects on task completion, and it is important to note that the experience of cognitive load is not the same in everyone. The elderly, students, and children experience different, and more often higher, amounts of cognitive load.
High cognitive load in the elderly has been shown to affect their center of balance.[4] With increased distractions and cell phone use students are more prone to experiencing high cognitive load which can reduce academic success.[5] Children have less general knowledge than adults which increases their cognitive load. Recent theoretical advances include the incorporation of embodied cognition in order to predict the cognitive load resulting from embodied interactions.[6]
Theory
"Cognitive load theory has been designed to provide guidelines intended to assist in the presentation of information in a manner that encourages learner activities that optimize intellectual performance".[7] Sweller's theory employs aspects of information processing theory to emphasize the inherent limitations of concurrent working memory load on learning during instruction. It makes use of the schema as primary unit of analysis for the design of instructional materials.
History
The history of cognitive load theory can be traced to the beginning of Cognitive Science in the 1950s and the work of G.A. Miller. In his classic paper,[8] Miller was perhaps the first to suggest our working memory capacity has inherent limits. His experimental results suggested that humans are generally able to hold only seven plus or minus two units of information in short-term memory. And in the early 1970s Simon and Chase[9] were the first to use the term "chunk" to describe how people might organize information in short-term memory. This chunking of memory components has also been described as schema construction.
In the late 1980s John Sweller developed cognitive load theory (CLT) while studying problem solving.[1] Studying learners as they solved problems, he and his associates found that learners often use a problem solving strategy called means-ends analysis. He suggests problem solving by means-ends analysis requires a relatively large amount of cognitive processing capacity, which may not be devoted to schema construction. Sweller suggests that instructional designers should prevent this unnecessary cognitive load by designing instructional materials which do not involve problem solving. Examples of alternative instructional materials include what are known as worked-examples and goal-free problems.
In the 1990s, cognitive load theory was applied in several contexts. The empirical results from these studies led to the demonstration of several learning effects: the completion-problem effect;[10] modality effect;[11][12] split-attention effect;[13] worked-example effect;[14][15] and expertise reversal effect.[16]
Types
Cognitive load theory provides a general framework and has broad implications for instructional design, by allowing instructional designers to control the conditions of learning within an environment or, more generally, within most instructional materials. Specifically, it provides empirically-based guidelines that help instructional designers decrease extraneous cognitive load during learning and thus refocus the learner's attention toward germane materials, thereby increasing germane (schema related) cognitive load. This theory differentiates between three types of cognitive load: intrinsic cognitive load, germane cognitive load, and extraneous cognitive load.[7]
Intrinsic
Intrinsic cognitive load is the inherent level of difficulty associated with a specific instructional topic. The term was first used in the early 1990s by Chandler and Sweller.[17] According to them, all instruction has an inherent difficulty associated with it (e.g., the calculation of 2 + 2, versus solving a differential equation). This inherent difficulty may not be altered by an instructor. However, many schemas may be broken into individual "subschemas" and taught in isolation, to be later brought back together and described as a combined whole.[18]
Extraneous
Extraneous cognitive load is generated by the manner in which information is presented to learners and is under the control of instructional designers.[17] This load can be attributed to the design of the instructional materials. Because there is a single, limited cognitive resource, using resources to process the extraneous load reduces the amount of resources available to process the intrinsic load and germane load (i.e., learning). Thus, especially when intrinsic and/or germane load is high (i.e., when a problem is difficult), materials should be designed so as to reduce the extraneous load.[19]
An example of extraneous cognitive load occurs when there are two possible ways to describe a square to a student.[20] A square is a figure and should be described using a figural medium. Certainly an instructor can describe a square in a verbal medium, but it takes just a second and far less effort to see what the instructor is talking about when a learner is shown a square, rather than having one described verbally. In this instance, the efficiency of the visual medium is preferred. This is because it does not unduly load the learner with unnecessary information. This unnecessary cognitive load is described as extraneous.
Chandler and Sweller introduced the concept of extraneous cognitive load. This article was written to report the results of six experiments that they conducted to investigate this working memory load. Many of these experiments involved materials demonstrating the split attention effect. They found that the format of instructional materials either promoted or limited learning. They proposed that differences in performance were due to higher levels of the cognitive load imposed by the format of instruction. "Extraneous cognitive load" is a term for this unnecessary (artificially induced) cognitive load.
Germane
Germane cognitive load is that load devoted to the processing, construction and automation of schemas. It was first described by Sweller, Van Merriënboer and Paas in 1998. While intrinsic cognitive load is generally thought to be immutable (although techniques can be applied to manage complexity by segmenting and sequencing complex material), instructional designers can manipulate extraneous and germane load. It is suggested that they limit extraneous load and promote germane load.[7]
Until the 1998 article by Sweller, Van Merriënboer & Paas, cognitive load theory primarily concentrated on the reduction of extraneous cognitive load. With this article, cognitive load researchers began to seek ways of redesigning instruction to redirect what would be extraneous load, to now be focused toward schema construction (germane load). Thus it is very important for instructional designers to "reduce extraneous cognitive load and redirect learners' attention to cognitive processes that are directly relevant to the construction of schemas".[21]
Measurement
Paas and Van Merriënboer[22] developed a construct (known as relative condition efficiency) which helps researchers measure perceived mental effort, an index of cognitive load. This construct provides a relatively simple means of comparing instructional conditions. It combines mental effort ratings with performance scores. Group mean z-scores are graphed and may be compared with a one-way Analysis of variance (ANOVA).
Paas and Van Merriënboer used relative condition efficiency to compare three instructional conditions (worked examples, completion problems, and discovery practice). They found learners who studied worked examples were the most efficient, followed by those who used the problem completion strategy. Since this early study many other researchers have used this and other constructs to measure cognitive load as it relates to learning and instruction.[23]
The ergonomic approach seeks a quantitative neurophysiological expression of cognitive load which can be measured using common instruments, for example using the heart rate-blood pressure product (RPP) as a measure of both cognitive and physical occupational workload.[24] They believe that it may be possible to use RPP measures to set limits on workloads and for establishing work allowance.
Task-invoked pupillary response is a form of measurement that directly reflects the cognitive load on working memory.[25] Greater pupil dilation is found to be associated with high cognitive load.[3] Pupil constriction occurs when there is low cognitive load.[3] Task-invoked pupillary response shows a direct correlation with working memory, making it an effective measurement of cognitive load explicitly unrelated to learning.
Some researchers have compared different measures of cognitive load. For example, Deleeuw and Mayer (2008)[26] compared three commonly used measures of cognitive load and found that they responded in different ways to extraneous, intrinsic, and germane load.
Individual differences in processing capacity
Evidence has been found that individuals systematically differ in their processing capacity.[27][28] For example, there are individual differences in processing capacities between novices and experts.[29] Experts have more knowledge or experience with regard to a specific task which reduces the cognitive load associated with the task. Novices do not have this experience or knowledge and thus have heavier cognitive load.
It has been theorized that an impoverished environment can contribute to cognitive load.[30] Regardless of the task at hand, or the processes used in solving the task, people who experience poverty also experience higher cognitive load. A number of factors contribute to the cognitive load in people with lower socioeconomic status that are not present in middle and upper-class people.[31]
Identifying the processing capacity of individuals could be extremely useful in further adapting instruction (or predicting the behavior) of individuals. Accordingly, further research would clearly be desirable. First, it is essential to compute the memory load imposed by detailed analysis of the processes to be used. Second, it is essential to ensure that individual subjects are actually using those processes. The latter requires intensive pre-training.
Effects of heavy cognitive load
A heavy cognitive load typically creates error or some kind of interference in the task at hand.[10][11][12][13][14][15][16] A heavy cognitive load can also increase stereotyping.[32] Stereotyping is an extension of the Fundamental Attribution Error which also increases in frequency with heavier cognitive load.[33] The notions of cognitive load and arousal contribute to the "Overload Hypothesis" explanation of social facilitation: in the presence of an audience, subjects tend to perform worse in subjectively complex tasks (whereas they tend to excel in subjectively easy tasks). See also: audience effect and drive theory.
Elderly
The danger of heavy cognitive load is seen in the elderly population. Aging can cause declines in the efficiency of working memory which can contribute to higher cognitive load.[34] The relationship between heavy cognitive load and control of center of mass are heavily correlated in the elderly population. As cognitive load increases, the sway in center of mass in elderly individuals increases.[35] Another study examined the relationship between body sway and cognitive function and their relationship during multitasking and found disturbances in balance led to a decrease in performance on the cognitive task[36] Heavy cognitive load can disturb balance in elderly people. Conversely, an increasing demand for balance can increase cognitive load.
Students
With the widespread acceptance of laptops in the classroom an increasing cognitive load while in school is a major concern. With the use of Facebook and other social forms of communication, adding multiple tasks is hurting students performance in the classroom. When many cognitive resources are available, the probability of switching from one task to another is high and does not lead to optimal switching behavior.[37] Both students who were heavy Facebook users and students who sat nearby those who were heavy Facebook users performed poorly and resulted in lower GPA.[38][39]
Children
The components of working memory as proposed by British psychologists, Alan Baddeley and Graham Hitch, are in place at 6 years of age.[40] However, there is a clear difference between adult and child knowledge. These differences are due to developmental increases in processing efficiency.[40] Children lack general knowledge, and this is what creates increased cognitive load in children. Children in impoverished families often experience even higher cognitive load in learning environments than those in middle-class families.[41] These children do not hear, talk, or learn about schooling concepts because their parents often do not have formal education. When it comes to learning, their lack of experience with numbers, words, and concepts increases their cognitive load.
As children grow older they develop superior basic processes and capacities.[41] They also develop metacognition, which helps them to understand their own cognitive activities.[41] Lastly, they gain greater content knowledge through their experiences.[41] These elements help reduce cognitive load in children as they develop.
Gesturing is a technique children use to reduce cognitive load while speaking.[42] By gesturing, they can free up working memory for other tasks.[42] Pointing allows a child to use the object they are pointing at as the best representation of it, which means they do not have to hold this representation in their working memory, thereby reducing their cognitive load.[43] Additionally, gesturing about an object that is absent reduces the difficulty of having to picture it in their mind.[44]
Embodiment, interactivity, and cognitive load
Cognitive load theorists have asked for updates that makes CLT more compatible with insights from embodied cognition research.[45] As a result, Embodied Cognitive Load Theory has been suggested as a means to predict the usefulness of interactive features in learning environments.[6] In this framework, the benefits of an interactive feature (such as easier cognitive processing) need to exceed its cognitive costs (such as motor coordination) in order for an embodied mode of interaction to increase learning outcomes.
Computational representation of mental workload
Human mental workload has gained importance, in the last few decades, as a fundamental design concept in human-computer interaction, education and other fields.[25] For people interacting with interfaces, computers and technological devices in general, the construct plays an important role. At a low level, while processing information, often people feel annoyed and frustrated; at higher level, mental workload is critical and dangerous as it leads to confusion, it decreases the performance of information processing and it increases the chances of errors and mistakes.[46] It is extensively documented that either mental overload or underload negatively affect performance. Hence, designers and practitioners who are ultimately interested in system or human performance need answers about operator workload at all stages of system design and operation.[47] At an early system design phase, designers require some explicit model to predict the mental workload imposed by their technologies on end-users so that alternative system designs can be evaluated. However, human mental workload is a multifaceted and complex construct mainly applied in cognitive sciences. A plethora of ad-hoc definitions can be found in the literature. Generally, it is not an elementary property, rather it emerges from the interaction between the requirements of a task, the circumstances under which it is performed and the skills, behaviours and perceptions of the operator. Although measuring mental workload has advantages in interaction and interface design, its formalisation as an operational and computational construct has not sufficiently been addressed. Many researchers agree that too many ad-hoc models are present in the literature and that they are applied subjectively by mental workload designers thereby limiting their application in different contexts and making comparison across different models difficult.
In a recent work,[48] the nature of the concept of mental workload, from a computational perspective, is argued to be a defeasible phenomenon. It is a concept built upon a set of reasons that can be defeated by additional reasons. The reasonable assumptions behind this are:
- Assumption 1: human mental workload is a complex construct built over a network of pieces of evidence;
- Assumption 2: accounting and understanding the relationships of accounted pieces of evidence as well as resolving the inconsistencies that might arise from their interaction is essential in modelling human mental workload.
The main contribution of this thesis [48] is the introduction of a methodology, developed as a formal modular framework, to represent mental workload as a defeasible computational concept and to assess it as a numerical usable index. The research contributes to the body of knowledge by providing a modular framework built upon Defeasible reasoning and formalised using Argumentation theory in which workload can be measured, analysed, explained and applied in different contexts. The framework follows the Popperian test of Falsifiability, this being also flexible and replicable. The preliminary solution proposed was designed for those scholars interested in engaging in the multi-disciplinary domain of mental workload and it is aimed at increasing its understanding and use. A further goal is to offer a new perspective on the formalisation of mental workload, encouraging further research on its representation, assessment and application in more general fields such as Human–computer interaction, Education, and Educational psychology.
See also
- Educational psychology
- Human factors and ergonomics
- Occupational stress
- Task-invoked pupillary response
- Task loading (in scuba diving)
- Information overload
- International Cognitive Load Theory Association (ICLTA)
References
- 1 2 Sweller, J (June 1988). "Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning". Cognitive Science 12 (2): 257–285. doi:10.1016/0364-0213(88)90023-7.
- ↑ Paas, Fred G. W. C.; Van Merriënboer, Jeroen J. G. (1993). "The Efficiency of Instructional Conditions: An Approach to Combine Mental Effort and Performance Measures". Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 35 (4): 737–743. doi:10.1177/001872089303500412.
- 1 2 3 GRANHOLM, ERIC; ASARNOW, ROBERT F.; SARKIN, ANDREW J.; DYKES, KAREN L. (July 1996). "Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations". Psychophysiology 33 (4): 457–461. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb01071.x.
- ↑ Andersson, Gerhard; Hagman, Jenni; Talianzadeh, Roya; Svedberg, Alf; Larsen, Hans Christian (May 2002). "Effect of cognitive load on postural control". Brain Research Bulletin 58 (1): 135–139. doi:10.1016/s0361-9230(02)00770-0.
- ↑ Frein, Scott T.; Jones, Samantha L.; Gerow, Jennifer E. (November 2013). "When it comes to Facebook there may be more to bad memory than just multitasking". Computers in Human Behavior 29 (6): 2179–2182. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.04.031.
- 1 2 Skulmowski, Alexander; Pradel, Simon; Kühnert, Tom; Brunnett, Guido; Rey, Günter Daniel. "Embodied learning using a tangible user interface: The effects of haptic perception and selective pointing on a spatial learning task". Computers & Education. 92–93: 64–75. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2015.10.011.
- 1 2 3 Sweller, J., Van Merriënboer, J., & Paas, F. (1998). "Cognitive architecture and instructional design". Educational Psychology Review 10 (3): 251–296. doi:10.1023/A:1022193728205.
- ↑ Miller, G.A. (1956). "The magic number seven plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity to process information". Psychological Review 63 (2): 81–97. doi:10.1037/h0043158. PMID 13310704.
- ↑ Chase, W.G. & Simon, H.A. (1973). "Perception in chess". Cognitive Psychology 4 (1): 55–81. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2.
- 1 2 Paas, F. (1992). "Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach". Journal of Educational Psychology 84 (4): 429–434. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.429.
- 1 2 Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. (1999). "Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: The role of modality and contiguity". Journal of Educational Psychology 91 (2): 358–368. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.2.358.
- 1 2 Mousavi, S., Low, R., & Sweller, J. (1995). "Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes". Journal of Educational Psychology 87 (2): 319–334. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.87.2.319.
- 1 2 Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1992). "The split-attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction". British Journal of Educational Psychology 62: 233–246. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1992.tb01017.x.
- 1 2 Cooper, G., & Sweller, J. (1987). "Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer". Journal of Educational Psychology 79 (4): 347–362. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.79.4.347.
- 1 2 Sweller, J., & Cooper, G.A. (1985). "The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra". Cognition and Instruction 2 (1): 59–89. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0201_3.
- 1 2 Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P. Chandler, P. and Sweller, J. (2003). "The Expertise Reversal Effect". Educational Psychologist 38 (1): 23–31. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3801_4.
- 1 2 Chandler, P. & Sweller, J. (1991). "Cognitive Load Theory and the Format of Instruction". Cognition and Instruction 8 (4): 293–332. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0804_2.
- ↑ Kirschner, P.A., Sweller, J., and Clark, R.E. (2006) Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: an analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist 41 (2) 75–86
- ↑ Ginns, P. (2006). "Integrating information: A meta-analysis of the spatial contiguity and temporal contiguity effects". Learning and Instruction 16 (6): 511–525. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.10.001.
- ↑ Clark, R., Nguyen, F., and Sweller, J. (2006). Efficiency in Learning: Evidence-Based Guidelines to Manage Cognitive Load. San Francisco: Pfeiffer. ISBN 0-7879-7728-4.
- ↑ (Sweller et al., 1998, p. 265)
- ↑ Paas, F.G.W.C., and Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (1993). "The efficiency of instructional conditions: An approach to combine mental-effort and performance measures". Human Factors 35 (4): 737–743.
- ↑ Paas, F., Tuovinen, J.E., Tabbers, H.K., & Van Gerven, P.W.M. (2003). "Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory". Educational Psychologist 38 (1): 63–71. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8.
- ↑ Fredericks T.K., Choi S.D., Hart J., Butt S.E., and Mital A. (2005). "An investigation of myocardial aerobic capacity as a measure of both physical and cognitive workloads". International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 35 (12): 1097–1107. doi:10.1016/j.ergon.2005.06.002.
- 1 2 Buettner, Ricardo (2013). Cognitive Workload of Humans Using Artificial Intelligence Systems: Towards Objective Measurement Applying Eye-Tracking Technology. KI 2013: 36th German Conference on Artificial Intelligence, September 16-20, 2013, Vol. 8077 of Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence (LNAI). Koblenz, Germany: Springer. pp. 37–48. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-40942-4_4.
- ↑ DeLeeuw, K.E., & Mayer, R.E. (2008). "A comparison of three measures of cognitive load: Evidence for separable measures of intrinsic, extraneous, and germane load.". Journal of Educational Psychology 100 (1): 223–234. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.223.
- ↑ Scandura, J.M. (1971). "Deterministic theorizing in structural learning: Three levels of empiricism". Journal of Structural Learning 3: 21–53.
- ↑ Voorhies, D. & Scandura, J.M. (1977). "7". Determination of memory load in information processing. Problem Solving, NY: Academic Press. pp. 299–316.
- ↑ Murphy, Gregory L.; Wright, Jack C. (1984). "Changes in conceptual structure with expertise: Differences between real-world experts and novices.". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 10 (1): 144–155. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.10.1.144.
- ↑ Mani, A.; Mullainathan, S.; Shafir, E.; Zhao, J. (29 August 2013). "Poverty Impedes Cognitive Function". Science 341 (6149): 976–980. doi:10.1126/science.1238041. PMID 23990553.
- ↑ Hackman, Daniel A.; Farah, Martha J. (February 2009). "Socioeconomic status and the developing brain". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13 (2): 65–73. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003.
- ↑ Biernat, Monica; Kobrynowicz, Diane; Weber, Dara L. (October 2003). "Stereotypes and Shifting Standards: Some Paradoxical Effects of Cognitive Load". Journal of Applied Social Psychology 33 (10): 2060–2079. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01875.x.
- ↑ Gilbert, D. T. (1989). Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 189–211). New York, Guilford Press.
- ↑ Wingfield, Arthur; Stine, Elizabeth A.L.; Lahar, Cindy J.; Aberdeen, John S. (June 1988). "Does the capacity of working memory change with age?". Experimental Aging Research 14 (2): 103–107. doi:10.1080/03610738808259731.
- ↑ Andersson, G.; Hagman, J.; Talianzadeh, R.; Svedberg, A.; Larsen, H. C. (2002). "Effect of cognitive load on postural control". Brain Research Bulletin 58 (1): 135–139. doi:10.1016/s0361-9230(02)00770-0.
- ↑ Faulkner, K. A., Redfern, M. S., Cauley, J. A., Landsittel, D. P., Studenski, S. A., Rosano, C., … Newman, A. B. (2007). Multitasking: Association between poorer performance and a history of recurrent falls. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 55(4), 570–576.
- ↑ Calderwood, C.; Ackerman, P. L.; Conklin, E. M. (2014). "What else do college students "do" while studying? An investigation of multitasking". Computers and Education 75: 19–29. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2014.02.004.
- ↑ Frein, S. T.; Jones, S. L.; Gerow, J. E. (2013). "When it comes to Facebook there may be more to bad memory than just multitasking". Computers in Human Behavior 29 (6): 2179–2182. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.04.031.
- ↑ Sana, F.; Weston, T.; Cepeda, N. J. (2013). "Laptop multitasking hinders classroom learning for both users and nearby peers". Computers and Education 62: 24–31. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2012.10.003.
- 1 2 Gathercole, Susan E.; Pickering, Susan J.; Ambridge, Benjamin; Wearing, Hannah (March 2004). "The Structure of Working Memory From 4 to 15 Years of Age.". Developmental Psychology 40 (2): 177–190. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177. PMID 14979759.
- 1 2 3 4 Alibali, Robert S. Siegler, Martha (2004). Children's thinking (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Pearson Education/Prentice Hall. ISBN 0131113844.
- 1 2 Ping, Raedy; Goldin-Meadow, Susan. "Gesturing Saves Cognitive Resources When Talking About Nonpresent Objects". Cognitive Science 34 (4): 602–619. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01102.x.
- ↑ Ballard, Dana H.; Hayhoe, Mary M.; Pook, Polly K.; Rao, Rajesh P. N. (1 December 1997). "Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (04): 723–42; discussion 743–67. doi:10.1017/s0140525x97001611. PMID 10097009.
- ↑ Ping, Raedy; Goldin-Meadow, Susan. "Gesturing Saves Cognitive Resources When Talking About Nonpresent Objects". Cognitive Science 34 (4): 602–619. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01102.x.
- ↑ Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2012). An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: Using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 24(1), 27-45.
- ↑ Buettner, Ricardo; Daxenberger, Barbara; Eckhardt, Andreas; Maier, Christian (2013). Cognitive Workload Induced by Information Systems: Introducing an Objective Way of Measuring based on Pupillary Diameter Responses. 12th Annual Pre-ICIS HCI/MIS Research Workshop, December 15, 2013, In Pre-ICIS HCI/MIS 2013 Proceedings, Paper 20. Milan, Italy: AIS. doi:10.13140/RG.2.1.3335.1129.
- ↑ Buettner, Ricardo; Sauer, Sebastian; Maier, Christian; Eckhardt, Andreas (2015). Towards ex ante Prediction of User Performance: A novel NeuroIS Methodology based on Real-Time Measurement of Mental Effort. 48th Annual Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences. Kauai, Hawaii: IEEE. pp. 533–542. doi:10.13140/2.1.4133.7603.
- 1 2 Longo, Luca (2014). Formalising Human Mental Workload as a Defeasible Computational Concept (Ph.D.). The University of Dublin, Trinity College.
Journal special issues
For those wishing to learn more about cognitive load theory, please consider reading these journals and special issues of those journals:
- Educational Psychologist, vol. 43 (2008) ISSN 0046-1520
- Applied Cognitive Psychology vol. 20(3) (2006)
- Applied Cognitive Psychology vol. 21(6) (2007)
- ETR&D vol. 53 (2005)
- Instructional Science vol. 32(1) (2004)
- Educational Psychologist vol. 38(1) (2003)
- Learning and Instruction vol. 12 (2002)
- Computers in Human Behavior vol. 25 (2) (2009)
For ergonomics standards see:
- ISO 10075-1:1991 Ergonomic Principles Related to Mental Workload – Part 1: General Terms and Definitions
- ISO 10075-2:1996 Ergonomic Principles Related To Mental Workload – Part 2: Design Principles
- ISO 10075-3:2004 Ergonomic Principles Related To Mental Workload – Part 3: Principles And Requirements Concerning Methods For Measuring And Assessing Mental Workload
- ISO 9241 Ergonomics of Human System Interaction
Further reading
- Barrett, H. C.; Frederick, D.; Haselton, M.; Kurzban, R. (2006). "Can manipulations of cognitive load be used to test evolutionary hypotheses?" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91 (3): 513–518. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.513. PMID 16938033.
- Cooper, G. (1990). "Cognitive load theory as an aid for instructional design". Australian Journal of Educational Technology 6 (2): 108–113.
- Cooper, Graham (1998). "Research into Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design at UNSW".
- Plass, J.L., Moreno, R., & Brünken, R., ed. (2010). Cognitive Load Theory. New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521677585.
- UNSW Cognitive Load Theory Conference - Sydney Australia 24-26 March 2007
- Measuring Cognitive Load using Linguistic Features – Implications for Usability Evaluation and Adaptive Interaction Design 21 Nov 2013
- Sweller, J. (1994). "Cognitive Load Theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design". Learning and Instruction 4 (4): 295–312. doi:10.1016/0959-4752(94)90003-5.
- Sweller, J. (1999). Instructional design in technical areas. Camberwell, Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research. ISBN 0-86431-312-8.
External links
- Cognitive Load Theory – a brief introduction to Cognitive load theory
- video of John Sweller explaining Extraneous load
- International Cognitive Load Theory Association (ICLTA)