Dark triad

The dark triad is a subject in psychology that focuses on three personality traits: narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy.[1][2][3][4] Use of the term "dark" implies that people possessing these traits have malevolent qualities:[5][6][7][8]

All three dark triad traits are conceptually distinct although empirical evidence shows them to be overlapping. They are associated with a callous-manipulative interpersonal style.[12] Additionally, psychopathy sometimes includes narcissism and Machiavellianism as possible traits.[11] A factor analysis carried out at the Glasgow Caledonian University found that among the big five personality traits, low agreeableness is the strongest correlate of the dark triad, while neuroticism and a lack of conscientiousness were associated with some of the dark triad members.[10]

Research on the dark triad is used in applied psychology, especially within the fields of law enforcement, clinical psychology and business management. People scoring high on these traits are more likely to commit crimes, cause social distress and create severe problems for an organization, especially if they are in a leadership position (for more information, see psychopathy, narcissism, and Machiavellianism in the workplace).

History

In 1998, McHoskey, Worzel, and Szyarto[13] provoked a controversy by claiming that narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy are more or less interchangeable in normal samples. Delroy L. Paulhus and McHoskey debated these perspectives at a subsequent American Psychological Association (APA) conference, inspiring a body of research that continues to grow in the published literature. Paulhus and Williams found enough behavioral, personality, and cognitive differences between the traits to suggest that they were distinct constructs; however, they concluded that further research was needed to elucidate how and why they overlap.[1]

Origins

The long-debated “nature versus nurture” issue has not escaped application to the dark triad. Research has begun to investigate the origins of the dark triad traits. In a similar manner to research on the Big Five, empirical studies have been conducted in an effort to understand the relative contributions of biology (nature) and environmental factors (nurture) in the development of dark triad traits.

One of the ways in which researchers attempt to dissect the relative influence of genetic and environmental factors on personality (and individual differences more generally) is a broad investigative technique loosely grouped under the heading of “twin studies.” For example, in one approach to twin studies,[14][15][16] researchers compare the personality scores of monozygotic (MZ) or identical twins reared together to dizygotic (DZ) or fraternal twins reared together. Because both types of twins in this design are reared together, all twin pairs are regarded as having shared a 100% common environment. In contrast, the monozygotic twins share 100% of their genes whereas the dizygotic twins only share about 50% of their genes. Therefore, for any given personality trait, it is possible to partial out genetic influences by first obtaining the MZ correlation (reflecting 100% common environment and 100% shared genes) and subtracting the DZ correlation (reflecting 100% common environment and 50% shared genes). This difference represents 50% of the genetic influence; doubled, this number is said to account for 100% of the genetic influence, and is one way to derive an index of heritability (sometimes called the heritability coefficient and represented as h2). Similarly, MZ - h2 may be regarded as an estimate of the influence of the common environment. Finally, because individual differences and the environment are supposed to account for the totality of behavior, it is said that subtracting the sum of h2 and the common environment influence from 1 is equal to the influence of unique or non-shared environments.

Biological

All three traits of the dark triad have been found to have substantial genetic components.[17] It has also been found that the observed relationships among the dark triad, and among the dark triad and the Big Five, are strongly driven by individual differences in genes.[18] However, while psychopathy (h2 = .64) and narcissism (h2 = .59) both have a relatively large heritable component, Machiavellianism (h2 = .31) has been found to be less heritable.[18][19]

Environmental

Compared to biological factors, the influence of environmental factors seem to be more subtle and account for less—yet still significant—variation in individual differences as related to the development of dark triad traits.[18] The influence of non-shared or unique environmental factors (definition and mathematical derivation included above at the end of the “Origins” subsection) accounts for a significant amount of the variance in all 3 dark triad traits (narcissism = .41, Machiavellianism = .30, psychopathy = .32), whereas only Machiavellianism (r = .39) has been found to be significantly related to a shared environmental factor.[20] Although it requires substantiation, some researchers have interpreted this latter finding (along with the comparatively lower heritability noted in the section above) to mean that Machiavellianism is the most likely dark triad trait to be influenced by experience.[21] At the very least, this notion about the modifiability of Machiavellianism does make some sense insofar as that the less variance there is attributable to genetic factors, the more variance there must be attributable to other factors, and "other" factors have traditionally be synopsized as environmental or experiential in nature.

Evolutionary

A third theory that has been referred to as instrumental in the development of dark triad traits, and which can be seen to influence both biological and environmental factors, is evolutionary theory. It has been argued that evolutionary behavior predicts not only the development of dark triad personalities, but also the flourishing of such personalities.[22] Indeed, it has been found that individuals demonstrating dark triad personality can be highly successful in society.[18] However, this success is typically short-lived.[18] The main evolutionary argument behind the dark triad traits emphasizes mating strategies.[23][24] This argument focuses on the notion of life history strategy.[25] Life history strategy proposes that individuals differ in reproductive strategies; an emphasis on mating is termed a “fast life” strategy, while an emphasis on parenting is termed a “slow reproductive” strategy.[25] There is some evidence [26][27] that the dark triad traits are related to fast life history strategies, however, there have been some mixed results, and not all three dark triad traits have been related to this strategy. A more detailed approach [28] has attempted to account for some of these mixed results by analyzing the traits at a finer level of detail. These researchers found that while some components of the dark triad are related to a fast life strategy, other components are related to slow reproductive strategies.[29]

Sub-clinical dimensions vs. disorders

In general, clinicians treat these traits as pathological, something that needs to be treated, and inherently undesirable, e.g. socially condemned or personally counter-productive. However, others argue that adaptive qualities may accompany the maladaptive ones. The evolutionary perspective (above) considers the dark triad to represent different mating strategies. Their frequency in the gene pool requires at least some local adaptation.

The everyday versions of these traits appear in student and community samples, where even high levels can be observed among individuals who manage to get along in daily life. Even in these samples, research indicates correlations with aggression,[30] racism,[31] and bullying[32] among other forms of social aversiveness.

Narcissism was discussed in the writings of Sigmund Freud, and psychopathy as a clinical diagnosis was addressed in the early writings of Hervey Cleckley in 1941 with the publication of The Mask of Sanity.[33] Given the dimensional model of narcissism and psychopathy, complemented by self-report assessments that are appropriate for the general population, these traits can now be studied at the subclinical level.[34] In the general population, the prevalence rates for sub-clinical and clinical psychopathy are estimated at around 1% and 0.2%, respectively.[35][36][37] Unfortunately, there do not seem to be any reliable estimates of either clinical or sub-clinical narcissism in the general population.[38]

With respect to empirical research, psychopathy was not formally studied until the 1970s with the pioneering efforts of Robert Hare, his Psychopathy Checklist (PCL), and its revision (PCL-R).[39] Hare notes in his book, Without Conscience [40] that asking psychopaths to self-report on psychologically important matters does not necessarily provide accurate or unbiased data. However, recent efforts have been made to study psychopathy in the dimensional realm using self-reported instruments, as with the Levenson Primary and Secondary Psychopathy Scales,[41] The Psychopathic Personality Inventory,[42] and the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale.[43]

Similarly, assessment of narcissism required clinical interviews, until the popular "Narcissistic Personality Inventory" was created by Raskin and Hall in 1979.[44] Since the NPI, several other measures have emerged which attempt to provide self-report alternatives for personality disorder assessment.[45] In addition, new instruments have been developed to study "pathological" narcissism [46] as opposed to "grandiose" narcissism, which is what many argue the NPI measures.[47][48]

Machiavellianism has never been referenced in any version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) for psychological disorders. It has been treated as strictly a personality construct. The original published version of the Mach-IV [49] is still the most widely used in empirical research.[50]

Components

As noted previously, there is a good deal of conceptual and empirical overlap between the dark triad traits. For example, researchers have noted that all three traits share characteristics such as a lack of empathy,[21] interpersonal hostility,[51] and interpersonal offensiveness.[52] Likely due in part to this overlap, a number of measures have recently been developed that attempt to measure all three dark triad traits simultaneously, such as the Dirty Dozen[53] and the short dark triad (SD3).[54]

At their root, however, most of these measures are questionnaire-style and require either self-response or observer-response (e.g., ratings from supervisors or coworkers). Both methods can prove problematic when attempting to measure any socially aversive trait as self-responders may be motivated to deceive.[55] A more specific confound might also exist for dark triad traits and Machiavellianism in particular: individuals who are skilled at deceiving and manipulating others should be perceived as low in deceptiveness and manipulation by others, and are therefore likely to receive inaccurate ratings.[56]

Despite these criticisms and the acknowledged commonalities among the dark triad traits, there is evidence that the constructs are related yet distinct.

Machiavellianism

Named after the philosophy of Niccolò Machiavelli, people who score high on this trait are cynical (in an amoral self-interest sense, not in a doubtful or skeptical sense), unprincipled, believe in interpersonal manipulation as the key for life success, and behave accordingly.[18] Scores on measures of Machiavellianism correlate negatively with agreeableness (r= -.47) and conscientiousness (r=-.34).[57] Machiavellianism is also significantly correlated with psychopathy.[58]

Narcissism

Individuals who score high on narcissism display grandiosity, entitlement, dominance, and superiority.[59] Narcissism has been found to correlate positively with extraversion (r = .42) and openness (r = .38) and negatively with agreeableness (r = -.36).[60] Narcissism has also been found to have a significant correlation with psychopathy.[61]

Psychopathy

Considered the most malevolent of the dark triad,[62] individuals who score high on psychopathy show low levels of empathy combined with high levels of impulsivity and thrill-seeking.[63] The similarity between psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder have been noted by some researchers.[18] Psychopathy has been found to correlate with all of the Big Five personality factors: extraversion (r = .34), agreeableness (r = .25), conscientiousness (r = .24), neuroticism (r = .34) and openness (r = .24).[64]

Group differences

The most pronounced group difference is in gender: numerous studies have shown that men tend to score higher than women on narcissism,[65] Machiavellianism,[66][67][68] and psychopathy,[41][42][69][70] although the magnitude of the difference varies across traits and the measurement instruments used. One interesting finding related to narcissism—albeit one based on non-representative samples—is that while men continue to score higher than women, it seems that the gender gap has shrunk considerably when comparing cohort data from 1992 and 2006. More specifically, the aforementioned findings indicate that there has been a general increase in levels of narcissism over time among college students of both sexes, but comparatively, the average level of narcissism in women has increased more than the average level of narcissism in men.[71]

There is far less information available on race differences in dark triad traits, and the data that is available is not representative of the population at-large. For instance, a 2008 research study using undergraduate participants found that Caucasians reported higher levels of narcissism relative to Asians.[72][73] Similarly, another 2008 study using undergraduate participants found that Caucasians tended to score slightly higher than non-Caucasians on Machiavellianism.[74] When attempting to discern whether there are ethnic differences in psychopathy, researchers have addressed the issue using different measurement instruments (e.g., the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale and The Psychopathic Personality Inventory), but no race differences have been found regardless of the measure used.[75][76] Additionally, when comparing Caucasians and African Americans from correctional, substance abuse, and psychiatric samples—groups with typically high prevalence rates of psychopathy—researchers again failed to find any meaningful group differences in psychopathy.[77] In summary, there is some sparse evidence that Caucasians tend to score higher on measures of narcissism and Machiavellianism than other ethnicities, while no evidence exists for race differences in psychopathy.

The focal variable when analyzing generational or cohort differences in dark triad traits has tended to be narcissism, arising from the hypothesis that so-called “Generation Me” or “Generation Entitlement” would exhibit higher levels of narcissism than previous generations. Indeed, based on analyses of responses to the Narcissistic Personality Inventory collected from over 16,000 U.S. undergraduate students between 1979 and 2006, it was concluded that average levels of narcissism had increased over time.[78] Similar results were obtained in a follow-up study that analyzed the changes within each college campus.[79] Trzesniewski & Donnellan (2010)[80] present conflicting evidence and argue that there have not been large changes in disposition or behavioral strategies across generations, although they do note that the current generation is less trusting and more cynical, which are both changes that might be indicative of an increase in Machiavellianism.

Perspectives

In the workplace

Oliver James identifies each of the three dark triadic personality traits as typically being prevalent in the workplace (see also Machiavellianism in the workplace, narcissism in the workplace and psychopathy in the workplace).[81] Furnham[82] (2010) has identified that the dark triad is related to the acquisition of leadership positions and interpersonal influence. In a meta-analysis of dark triad and workplace outcomes, Jonason and colleagues (2012) found that each of the dark triad traits were related to manipulation in the workplace, but each via unique mechanisms. Specifically, Machiavellianism was related with the use of excessive charm in manipulation, narcissism was related with the use of physical appearance, and psychopathy was related with physical threats.[83] Jonason and colleagues also found that the dark triad traits fully mediated the relationship between gender and workplace manipulation. The dark triad traits have also been found to be fairly well-represented in upper-level management and CEOs.[84]

Internet trolls

Main article: Troll (Internet)

Recent studies have found that people who are identified as trolls tend to have dark personality traits and show signs of sadism, antisocial behavior, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism.[85][86][87] The 2013 case study suggested that there are a number of similarities between anti-social and flame trolling activities and the 2014 survey indicated that trolling is an Internet manifestation of everyday sadism. Both studies suggest that this trolling may be linked to bullying in both adolescents and adults.

As a mating strategy

Studies have suggested that on average, those who exhibit the dark triad of personality traits have an accelerated mating strategy, reporting more sex partners, more favorable attitudes towards casual sex,[88] lowered standards in their short-term mates,[89] a tendency to steal or poach mates from others,[90] more risk-taking in the form of substance abuse,[91] a tendency to prefer immediate but smaller amounts of money over delayed but larger amounts of money,[92] limited self-control and greater incidence of ADHD symptoms[93] and a pragmatic and game-playing love style.[94] These traits have been identified as part of a fast life strategy that appears to be enacted by an exploitative, opportunistic, and protean approach to life in general[95] and at work.[96]

The evidence is mixed regarding the exact link between the dark triad and reproductive success. For example, there is a lack of empirical evidence for reproductive success in the case of psychopathy.[11] Additionally, these traits are not universally short-term-oriented[97] nor are they all impulsive.[98] Furthermore, much of the research reported pertaining to the dark triad cited in the above paragraph is based on statistical procedures that assume the dark triad are a single construct, in spite of genetic[99] and meta-analytic evidence to the contrary.[100]

Appearance

Several academic studies have found evidence that people with dark triad personalities are judged as slightly better-looking than average on first sight.[101] Two studies have determined that this is because people with dark triad traits put more effort into their appearance, and the difference in attractiveness disappears when "dressed down" with bland clothing and without make up.[102][103] Two more studies found that only narcissistic subjects were judged to be better-looking, but the other dark triad traits of machiavellianism and psychopathy had no correlation with looks.[104][105]

Related concepts

The Big Five

The five factor model of personality has significant relationships with the dark triad combined and with each of the dark triad’s traits. The dark triad overall is negatively related to both agreeableness and conscientiousness.[106] More specifically, Machiavellianism captures a suspicious versus trusting view of human nature which is also captured by the Trust sub-scale on the agreeableness trait.[107] Extroversion captures similar aspects of assertiveness, dominance, and self-importance as narcissism.[107] Narcissism also is positively related to the achievement striving and competence aspects of Conscientiousness. Psychopathy has the strongest correlations with low dutifulness and deliberation aspects of Conscientiousness.[106]

Honesty-humility

The honesty-humility factor from the HEXACO model of personality is used to measure sincerity, fairness, greed avoidance, and modesty. Honesty-Humility contrasts antisocial behavior at the low end of the trait with pro-social behavior at the high end which strongly theoretically links it to the dark triad traits.[108] Likewise, all three dark triad traits are strongly negatively correlated with Honesty-Humility.[106] The conceptual overlap of the three traits which represents a tendency to manipulate and exploit others for personal gain defines the negative pole of the honesty-humility factor.[109] · Typically, any positive effects from the DT and low H-H occur at the individual level, that is, any benefits are conferred onto the beholder of the traits (e.g., successful mating, obtainment of leadership positions) and not onto others or society at large. However, one positive outcome of low H-H, that while still providing benefits to the individual, also indirectly benefits society, is that of creativity. A study by Silvia and colleagues (2011) found that individuals who score low in Honesty-Humility have higher levels of creativity.[110]

Dark tetrad

Several researchers have suggested expanding the dark triad to contain a fourth dark trait. Everyday sadism, defined as the enjoyment of cruelty, is the most common addition. While sadism is highly correlated with the dark triad, researchers have shown that sadism predicts anti-social behavior beyond the dark triad.[111][112] Borderline personality disorder and status-driven risk-taking have also been proposed as additions.[106]

Vulnerable dark triad

The vulnerable dark triad (VDT) comprises three related and similar constructs: vulnerable narcissism, factor 2 psychopathy, and borderline personality disorder. A study found that these three constructs are significantly related to one another, and manifest similar nomological networks. Although the VDT members are related to negative emotionality and antagonistic interpersonal styles, they are also related to introversion and disinhibition.[113]

See also

References

  1. 1 2 Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K.M. (2002). The Dark Triad of Personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 556-563.
  2. Robert M. Regoli; John D. Hewitt; Matt DeLisi (20 April 2011). Delinquency in Society: The Essentials. Jones & Bartlett Learning. p. 99. ISBN 978-0-7637-7790-6.
  3. W. Keith Campbell; Joshua D. Miller (7 July 2011). The Handbook of Narcissism and Narcissistic Personality Disorder: Theoretical Approaches, Empirical Findings, and Treatments. John Wiley & Sons. p. 154. ISBN 978-1-118-02924-4.
  4. Mark R. Leary; Rick H. Hoyle (5 June 2009). Handbook of individual differences in social behavior. Guilford Press. p. 100. ISBN 978-1-59385-647-2.
  5. Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic; Sophie von Stumm; Adrian Furnham (23 February 2011). The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences. John Wiley & Sons. p. 527. ISBN 978-1-4443-4310-6.
  6. Paulhus, D. L., Williams, K. M. (2002). "The Dark Triad of personality: narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Journal of Research in Personality 36 (6): 556–63. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6.
  7. Leonard M. Horowitz; Stephen Strack, Ph.D. (14 October 2010). Handbook of Interpersonal Psychology: Theory, Research, Assessment and Therapeutic Interventions. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 252–55. ISBN 978-0-470-88103-3. Retrieved 15 February 2012.
  8. David Lacey (17 March 2009). Managing the Human Factor in Information Security: How to Win Over Staff and Influence Business Managers. John Wiley & Sons. p. 248. ISBN 978-0-470-72199-5.
  9. Kohut, H. (1977). The Restoration of the Self. New York: International Universities Press.
  10. 1 2 Jakobwitz, S., Egan, V. (2006). "The 'dark triad' and normal personality traits". Personality and Individual Differences 40 (2): 331–39. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.07.006.
  11. 1 2 3 Skeem, J. L.; Polaschek, D. L. L.; Patrick, C. J.; Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). "Psychopathic Personality: Bridging the Gap Between Scientific Evidence and Public Policy". Psychological Science in the Public Interest 12 (3): 95–162. doi:10.1177/1529100611426706.
  12. Jones, D. N., Paulhus, D. L. (2010). "Differentiating the dark triad within the interpersonal circumplex". In Horowitz, L. M., Strack, S. N. Handbook of interpersonal theory and research. New York: Guilford. pp. 249–67.
  13. McHoskey J. W., Worzel W., Szyarto C. (1998). "Machiavellianism and psychopathy". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 (1): 192–210. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.192. PMID 9457782.
  14. Petrides, K. V.; Vernon, P.A.; Schermer, J.A.; Veselka, L (2011). "Trait emotional intelligence and the dark triad traits of personality". Twin Research and Human Genetics 14 (1): 35.
  15. Vernon, P.A.; Martin, R.A.; Schermer, J.A.; Mackie, A. (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of humor styles and their correlations with the Big-5 personality dimensions". Personality and Individual Differences 44 (5): 1116–1125.
  16. Vernon, P.A.; Villani, V.C.; Vickers, L.C.; Harris, J.A. (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2): 445–452.
  17. Petrides, K. V.; Vernon, P.A.; Schermer, J.A.; Veselka, L (2011). "Trait emotional intelligence and the dark triad traits of personality". Twin Research and Human Genetics 14 (1): 35.
  18. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Furnham, A.; Richards, S.C.; Paulhus, D.L. (2013). "The Dark Triad of personality: A 10 year review". Social and Personality Psychology Compass 7 (3): 199–216.
  19. Vernon, P.A.; Villani, V.C.; Vickers, L.C.; Harris, J.A. (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2): 445–452.
  20. Vernon, P.A.; Martin, R.A.; Schermer, J.A.; Mackie, A. (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of humor styles and their correlations with the Big-5 personality dimensions". Personality and Individual Differences 44 (5): 1116–1125.
  21. 1 2 Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. (2011). "The role of impulsivity in the dark triad of personality". Personality and Individual Differences 51 (5): 679–682.
  22. Mealey, L. (1995). "The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary model". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (03): 523–541.
  23. Jonason, P.K.; Webster, G.D.; Schmitt, D.P. (2009). "The dark triad: Facilitating a short-term mating strategy in men". European Journal of Personality 23 (1): 5–18.
  24. Brumbach, B.H.; Figueredo, A.J.; Ellis, B.J. (2009). "Effects of harsh and unpredictable environments in adolescence on development of life history strategies". Human Nature 20 (1): 25–51.
  25. 1 2 Rushton, J.P. (1985). "Differential K theory: The sociobiology of individual and group differences". Personality and Individual Differences 6 (4): 441–452.
  26. Jonason, P.K.; Tost, J. (2010). "I just cannot control myself: The Dark Triad and self-control". Personality and Individual Differences 49 (6): 611–615.
  27. Jonason, P.K.; Koenig, B.L.; Tost, J. (2010). "Living a fast life". Human Nature 21 (4): 428–442.
  28. McDonald, M.M.; Donnellan, M.B.; Navarrete, C.D. (2012). "A life history approach to understanding the Dark Triad". Personality and Individual Differences 52 (5): 601–605.
  29. McDonald, M.M.; Donnellan, M.B.; Navarrete, C.D. (2012). "A life history approach to understanding the Dark Triad". Personality and Individual Differences 52 (5): 601–605.
  30. Jones D.N., Paulhus D. L. (2010). "Different provocations trigger aggression in narcissists and psychopaths". Social Psychological and Personality Science 1: 12–18. doi:10.1177/1948550609347591.
  31. Hodson, G. M., Hogg, S. M., & MacInnis, C. C. (2009). The role of "dark personalities" (narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy), Big Five personality factors, and ideology in explaining prejudice. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 686–690.
  32. Chabrol H., Van Leeuwen N., Rodgers R., Séjourné S. (2009). "Contributions of psychopathic, narcissistic, Machiavellian, and sadistic personality traits to juvenile delinquency". Personality and Individual Differences 47 (7): 734–39. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.020.
  33. Cleckly, H. C. (1941). The Mask of Sanity: An Attempt to Reinterpret the So-Called Psychopathic Personality. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
  34. LeBreton, J. M., Binning, J. F., & Adorno, A. J. (2005). Sub-clinical psychopaths. In D. L. Segal & J. C. Thomas (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of personality and psychopathology: Vol. 1. Personality and everyday functioning (pp. 388–411). New York: John Wiley.
  35. Babiak, P., Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2010). Corporate psychopathy: Talking the walk. Behavioral Sciences & The Law, 28, 174–193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ bsl.925
  36. Coid, J., Yang, M., Ullrich, S., Roberts, A., & Hare, R. D. (2009). Prevalence and correlates of psychopathic traits in the household population of Great Britain. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 32, 65–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.ijlp.2009.01.002.
  37. Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2008). Psychopathic traits in a large community sample: Links to violence, alcohol use, and intelligence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 893–899. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.76.5.893.
  38. Foster, J. D., & Campbell, W. K. (2007). Are there such things as “narcissists” in social psychology? A taxometric analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(6), 1321-1332.
  39. Hare, R.D., (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.
  40. Hare, R. D. (1999). Without conscience: The disturbing world of the psychopaths among us. New York: Guilford Press.
  41. 1 2 Levenson M. R., Kiehl K. A., Fitzpatrick C. M. (1995). "Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68 (1): 151–58. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.151. PMID 7861311.
  42. 1 2 Lilienfeld S. O., Andrews B. P. (1996). "Development and preliminary validation of a self-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncrimnal population". Journal of Personality Assessment 66 (3): 488–524. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6603_3. PMID 8667144.
  43. Paulhus, D. L., Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2015). Manual for the Self-Report Psychopathy scales (4th ed.). Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
  44. Raskin R., Hall C. S. (1979). "A Narcissistic Personality Inventory". Psychological Reports 45 (2): 590. doi:10.2466/pr0.1979.45.2.590.
  45. Hyler, S.E. (1994). Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire-4 (Unpublished test). New York: NYSPI.
  46. Pincus A. L., Ansell E. B., Pimentel C. A., Cain N. M., Wright A. G. C., Levy K. N. (2009). "Initial construction and validation of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory". Psychological Assessment 21 (3): 365–79. doi:10.1037/a0016530. PMID 19719348.
  47. Miller, J. D., & Campbell, W. K. (2008). Comparing clinical and Social-Personality Conceptutalizations of narcissism. Journal of Personality, 76, 449–476.
  48. Wink P (1991). "Two faces of narcissism". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61 (4): 590–97. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.4.590. PMID 1960651.
  49. Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press.
  50. Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2009). Machiavellianism. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior (pp. 93–108). New York: Guilford.
  51. Lynam, D.R.; Gaughan, E.T. (2011). "Assessing the basic traits associated with psychopathy: Development and validation of the Elemental Psychopathy Assessment". Psychological Assessment 23 (1).
  52. Egan, V.; McCorkindale, C. (2007). "Narcissism, vanity, personality and mating effort". Personality and Individual Differences 43 (8): 2105–2115.
  53. Jonason, P.K.; Webster, G.D. (210). "The dirty dozen: a concise measure of the dark triad". Psychological assessment 22 (2): 420.
  54. Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. (2014). "Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3) A Brief Measure of Dark Personality Traits". Assessment 21 (1): 28–41.
  55. O'Boyle, E.H.; Forsyth, D.R.; Banks, G.C.; McDaniel, M.A. (2012). "A meta-analysis of the dark triad and work behavior: A social exchange perspective". Journal of Applied Psychology 97 (3): 557.
  56. O'Boyle, E.H.; Forsyth, D.R.; Banks, G.C.; McDaniel, M.A. (2012). "A meta-analysis of the dark triad and work behavior: A social exchange perspective". Journal of Applied Psychology 97 (3): 557.
  57. Paulhus, D.L.; Williams, K.M. (2002). "The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Journal of research in personality 36 (6): 556–563.
  58. Vernon, P.A.; Villani, V.C.; Vickers, L.C.; Harris, J.A. (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2): 445–452.
  59. Corry, N.; Merritt, R.D.; Mrug, S.; Pamp, B. (2008). "The factor structure of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory". Journal of Personality Assessment 90 (6): 593–600.
  60. Paulhus, D.L.; Williams, K.M. (2002). "The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Journal of research in personality 36 (6): 556–563.
  61. Vernon, P.A.; Villani, V.C.; Vickers, L.C.; Harris, J.A. (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2): 445–452.
  62. Rauthmann, J.F. (2012). "The Dark Triad and interpersonal perception: Similarities and differences in the social consequences of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Social Psychological and Personality Science 3 (4): 487–496.
  63. Hare, R.D. (1985). "Comparison of procedures for the assessment of psychopathy". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 53 (1): 7.
  64. Vernon, P.A.; Villani, V.C.; Vickers, L.C.; Harris, J.A. (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2): 445–452.
  65. Twenge, Jean; Konrath, Sarah; Foster, Joshua; Campbell, Keith; Bushman, Brad (2008). "Egos inflating over time: a cross‐temporal meta‐analysis of the Narcissistic personality inventory". Journal of Personality 76 (4): 875–902. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00507.x.
  66. Chonko, L.B. (1982). "). Machiavellianism: Sex differences in the profession of purchasing management". Psychological Reports 51 (2): 645–646.
  67. Dahling, J.J.; Whitaker, B.G.; Levy, P.E. (2008). "The development and validation of a new Machiavellianism scale". doi:10.1177/0149206308318618.
  68. Wertheim, E.G.; Widom, C.S.; Wortzel, L.H. (1978). "Multivariate analysis of male and female professional career choice correlates". Journal of Applied Psychology 63 (2): 234.
  69. Cale, E.M.; Lilienfeld, S.O. (2002). "Sex differences in psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder: A review and integration". Clinical psychology review 22 (8): 1179–1207.
  70. Zagon, I.K.; Jackson, H.J. (1994). "Construct validity of a psychopathy measure". Journal of Personality and Individual Differences: 125–135.
  71. Twenge, Jean; Konrath, Sarah; Foster, Joshua; Campbell, Keith; Bushman, Brad (2008). "Egos inflating over time: a cross‐temporal meta‐analysis of the Narcissistic personality inventory". Journal of Personality 76 (4): 875–902. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00507.x.
  72. Trzesniewski, K.H.; Donnellan, M.B. (2008). "Do today's young people really think they are so extraordinary? An examination of secular trends in narcissism and self-enhancement". Psychological Science 19 (2): 181–188.
  73. Twenge, J.M.; Foster, J.D. (2008). "Mapping the scale of the narcissism epidemic: Increases in narcissism 2002–2007 within ethnic groups". Journal of Research in Personality 42 (6): 1619–1622.
  74. Dahling, J.J.; Whitaker, B.G.; Levy, P.E. (2008). "The development and validation of a new Machiavellianism scale". doi:10.1177/0149206308318618.
  75. Epstein, M.K.; Poythress, N.G.; Brandon, K.O. (2006). "The Self-Report Psychopathy Scale and Passive Avoidance Learning A Validation Study of Race and Gender Effects". Assessment 13 (2): 197–207.
  76. Lander, G.C.; Lutz-ZOis, C.J.; Rye, M.S.; Goodnight, J.A. (2012). "The differential association between alexithymia and primary versus secondary psychopathy". Personality and Individual Differences 52 (1): 45–50.
  77. Skeem, J.L.; Edens, J.F.; Camp, J.; Colwell, L.H. (2004). "Are there ethnic differences in levels of psychopathy? A meta-analysis". Law and human behavior 28 (5): 505.
  78. Twenge, Jean; Konrath, Sarah; Foster, Joshua; Campbell, Keith; Bushman, Brad (2008). "Egos inflating over time: a cross‐temporal meta‐analysis of the Narcissistic personality inventory". Journal of Personality 76 (4): 875–902. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00507.x.
  79. Twenge, J.M.; Campbell, W.K. (2010). "Birth Cohort Differences in the Monitoring the Future Dataset and Elsewhere Further Evidence for Generation Me—Commentary on Trzesniewski & Donnellan". Perspectives on Psychological Science 5 (1): 81–88.
  80. Trzesniewski, K.H.; Donnellan, M.B. (2010). "Rethinking “Generation Me” a study of cohort effects from 1976–2006". Perspectives on Psychological Science 5 (1): 58–75.
  81. James O Office Politics: How to Thrive in a World of Lying, Backstabbing and Dirty Tricks (2013)
  82. Furnham, Adrian (2010). "The dark triad of personality: A ten year review". Social and personality psychology compass.
  83. Jonason, P (2012). "The Dark Triad at work: How toxic employees get their way". Personality and Individual Differences.
  84. Amernic, J. (2010). "Accounting as a facilitator of extreme narcissism". Journal of Business Ethics.
  85. Buckels, E.E., Trapnell, P.D., & Paulhus, D.L. (2013). Trolls Just Want to Have Fun. Psychological Science, 24, 2201-2209.
  86. Bishop, J. (2013). The effect of de-individuation of the Internet Troller on Criminal Procedure implementation: An interview with a Hater, 7, 28–48. International Journal of Cyber Criminology. http://www.cybercrimejournal.com/Bishop2013janijcc.pdf
  87. "Internet Trolls Are Narcissists, Psychopaths, and Sadists".
  88. Jonason P. K., Li N. P., Webster G. W., Schmitt D. P. (2009). "The Dark Triad: Facilitating short-term mating in men". European Journal of Personality 23: 5–18. doi:10.1002/per.698.
  89. Jonason P. K., Valentine K. A., Li N. P., Harbeson C. L. (2011). "Mate-selection and the Dark Triad: Facilitating a short-term mating strategy and creating a volatile environment". Personality and Individual Differences 51 (6): 759–63. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.025.
  90. Jonason P. K., Li N. P., Buss D. M. (2010). "The costs and benefits of the Dark Triad: Implications for mate poaching and mate retention tactics". Personality and Individual Differences 48 (4): 373–78. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.003.
  91. Jonason P. K., Koenig B., Tost J. (2010). "Living a fast life: The Dark Triad and Life History Theory". Human Nature 21 (4): 428–42. doi:10.1007/s12110-010-9102-4.
  92. Jonason, P. K., Li N. P., & Teicher, E. A. (2010). Who is James Bond?:The Dark Triad as an agentic social style. Individual Differences Research, 8, 111–120.
  93. Jonason, P. K., & Tost, J. (2010). I just cannot control myself: The Dark Triad and self-control. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 611–615.
  94. Jonason, P.K., & Kavanagh, P. (2010). The dark side of love: The Dark Triad and love styles. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 606–610.
  95. Jonason P.K., Webster G.D. (2012). "A protean approach to social influence: Dark Triad Personalities and Social Influence Tactics". Personality and Individual Differences 52 (4): 521–26. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.023.
  96. Jonason P. K., Slomski S., Partyka J. (2012). "The Dark Triad at work: How toxic employees get their way". Personality and Individual Differences 52 (3): 449–53. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.008.
  97. Jonason, P. K. & Tost, J. (2010). I just cannot control myself: The Dark Triad and self-control. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 611–615.
  98. Jones D. N., Paulhus D. L. (2011). "The role of impulsivity in the Dark Triad of personality". Personality and Individual Differences 51 (5): 679–82. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.011.
  99. Vernon P. A., Villani V. C., Vickers L. C., Harris J. A. (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big Five". Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2): 445–52. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.007.
  100. O'Boyle E. H., Forsyth D. R., Banks G. C., McDaniel M. A. (2012). "A meta-analysis of the Dark Triad and work behavior: A social exchange perspective". Journal of Applied Psychology 97 (3): 557–79. doi:10.1037/a0025679. PMID 22023075.
  101. Carter, Gregory Louis, Anne C. Campbell, Steven Muncer, "The Dark Triad personality: Attractiveness to women", Elsevier/ScienceDirect, 12 June 2013
  102. Grewel, Daisy, "Psychology Uncovers Sex Appeal of Dark Personalities", Scientific American, 27 November 2012
  103. Holtzman, Nicholas, S., "People With Dark Personalities Tend to Create a Physically Attractive Veneer" Social Psychological and Personality Science, October 4, 2012, doi: 10.1177/1948550612461284
  104. "Are Narcissists Sexy? Zeroing in on the Effect of Narcissism on Short-Term Mate Appeal", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, July 2013 vol. 39 no. 7 870-882, doi: 10.1177/0146167213483580
  105. Back, Schmuckle, and Egloff, "Why are narcissists so charming at first sight? Decoding the narcissism-popularity link at zero acquaintance.", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2010 Jan;98(1):132-45. doi: 10.1037/a0016338
  106. 1 2 3 4 Furnham, Adrian; Richards, Steven; Paulhus, Delroy (2013). "The Dark Triad of Personality: A 10 Year Review". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. doi:10.1111/spc3.12018.
  107. 1 2 Hunter, J. E.; Gerbing, D. W.; Boster, F. J. (1982). "Machiavellian beliefs and personality: Construct invalidity of the Machiavellianism dimension". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (43): 1293–1305.
  108. Aghababaei, N., Mohammadtabar, S., & Saffarinia, M. (2014). Dirty Dozen vs. the H factor: Comparison of the Dark Triad and Honesty–Humility in prosociality, religiosity, and happiness. Personality and Individual Differences,67, 6-10.
  109. Lee, K., Ashton, M. C., Wiltshire, J., Bourdage, J. S., Visser, B. A., & Gallucci, A. (2013). Sex, power, and money: Prediction from the Dark Triad and Honesty–Humility. European Journal of Personality, 27(2), 169-184.
  110. Silvia, P; Kaufman, J (2011). "Cantankerous creativity: Honesty–Humility, Agreeableness, and the HEXACO structure of creative achievement". Personality and Individual Differences.
  111. Chabrol H., Van Leeuwen, N., Rodgers, R., & Sejourne, N. (2009). Contributions of psychopathic, narcissistic, Machiavellian, and sadistic personality traits to juvenile delinquency. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 734–739.
  112. Buckels, E.E., Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). Psychological Science, 24, 2201-2209.
  113. Miller, JD.; Dir, A.; Gentile, B.; Wilson, L.; Pryor, LR.; Campbell, K. (October 2010). "Searching for a Vulnerable Dark Triad: Comparing Factor 2 Psychopathy, Vulnerable Narcissism, and Borderline Personality Disorder" (PDF). Journal of Personality 78 (5): 1529–1564. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00660.x.

External links

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Wednesday, May 04, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.