Dichotic listening test
Dichotic Listening Test | |
---|---|
Diagnostics | |
MeSH | D004007 |
Dichotic listening is a psychological test commonly used to investigate selective attention within the auditory system and is a subtopic of cognitive psychology and neuroscience. Specifically, it is "used as a behavioral test for hemispheric lateralization of speech sound perception."[1] During a standard dichotic listening test, a participant is presented with two different auditory stimuli simultaneously (usually speech). The different stimuli are directed into different ears over headphones.[2] Research Participants were instructed to repeat aloud the words they heard in one ear while a different message was presented to the other ear. As a result of focusing to repeat the words, participants noticed little of the message to the other ear, often not even realizing that at some point it changed from English to German. At the same time, participants did notice when the voice in the unattended ear changed from a male’s to a female’s, suggesting that the selectivity of consciousness can work to tune in some information."[3]
Test designs
Dichotic Fused Words Test (DFWT)
The "Dichotic Fused Words Test" (DFWT) is a modified version of the basic dichotic listening test. It was originally explored by Johnson et al. (1977)[4] but in the early 80's Wexler and Hawles (1983)[5] modified this original test to ascertain more accurate data pertaining to hemispheric specialization of language function. In the DFWT, each participant listens to pairs of monosyllabic rhyming consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) words. Each word varies in the initial consonant. The significant difference in this test is "the stimuli are constructed and aligned in such a way that partial interaural fusion occurs: subjects generally experience and report only one stimulus per trial."[6] According to Zatorre (1989), some major advantages of this method include "minimizing attentional factors, since the percept is unitary and localized to the midline" and "stimulus dominance effects may be explicitly calculated, and their influence on ear asymmetries assessed and eliminated."[6] Wexler and Hawles study obtained a high test-retest reliability (r=0.85).[5] High test-retest reliability is good, because it proves that the data collected from the study is consistent.
Testing with Emotional Factors
An emotional version of the dichotic listening task was developed. In this version individuals listen to the same word in each ear but they hear it in either a surprised, happy, sad, angry, or neutral tone. Participants are then asked to press a button indicating what tone they heard. Usually dichotic listening tests show a right-ear advantage for speech sounds. Right-ear/left-hemisphere advantage is expected, because of evidence from Broca's area and Wernicke's area, which are both located in the left hemisphere in most of right-handed people. In contrast, the left ear (and therefore the right hemisphere) is often better at processing nonlinguistic material.[7] The data from the emotional dichotic listening task is consistent with the other studies, because participants tend to have more correct responses to their left ear than to the right.[8] It is important to note that the emotional dichotic listening task is seemingly harder for the participants than the phonemic dichotic listening task. Meaning more incorrect responses were submitted by individuals.
Manipulation of Voice Onset Time (VOT)
The manipulation of voice onset time (VOT) during dichotic listening tests have given many insights regarding brain function.[9] To date, the most common design is the utilisation of four VOT conditions: short-long pairs (SL), where a Consonant-Vowel (CV) syllable with a short VOT is presented to the left ear and a CV syllable with a long VOT is presented to the right ear, as well as long-short (LS), short-short (SS) and long-long (LL) pairs. In 2006, Rimol et al.[10] first reported that in healthy adults SL pairs elicit the largest right ear advantage (REA) while, in fact, LS pairs elicit a significant left ear advantage (LEA). A study of children 5–8 years old has shown a developmental trajectory whereby long VOTs gradually start to dominate over short VOTs when LS pairs are being presented under dichotic conditions.[11] Converging evidence from studies of attentional modulation of the VOT effect shows that around age 9 children lack the adult-like cognitive flexibility required to exert top-down control over stimulus-driven bottom-up processes.[12] Arciuli et al.(2010) further demonstrated that this kind of cognitive flexibility is a predictor of proficiency with complex tasks such as reading.[9][13]
Neuroscience
Dichotic listening tests can also be used as lateralized speech assessment task. Neuropsychologists have used this test to explore the role of singular neuroanatomical structures in speech perception and language asymmetry. For example, Hugdahl et al. (2003), investigated dichotic listening performance and frontal lobe function[14] in left and right lesioned frontal lobe nonaphasiac patients compared to healthy controls. In the study, all groups were exposed to 36 dichotic trials with pairs of CV syllables and each patient was asked to state which syllable he or she heard best. As expected, the right lesioned patients showed a right ear advantage like the healthy control group but the left hemisphere lesioned patients displayed impairment when compared to both the right lesioned patients and control group. From this study, researchers concluded "dichotic listening as into a neuronal circuitry which also involves the frontal lobes, and that this may be a critical aspect of speech perception."[14] Similarly, Westerhausen and Hugdahl (2008)[15] analyzed the role of the corpus callosum in dichotic listening and speech perception. After reviewing many studies, it was concluded that "...dichotic listening should be considered a test of functional inter-hemispheric interaction and connectivity, besides being a test of lateralized temporal lobe language function" and "the corpus callosum is critically involved in the top-down attentional control of dichotic listening performance, thus having a critical role in auditory laterality."[15]
Language Processing
Dichotic listening can also be used to test the hemispheric asymmetry of language processing. In the early 60s, Doreen Kimura reported that dichotic verbal stimuli (specifically spoken numerals) presented to a participant produced a right ear advantage (REA).[16] She attributed the right-ear advantage "to the localization of speech and language processing in the so-called dominant left hemisphere of the cerebral cortex." [17] According to her study, this phenomenon was related to the structure of the auditory nerves and the left-sided dominance for language processing.[18] It is important to note that REA doesn't apply to non-speech sounds. In "Hemispheric Specialization for Speech Perception," by Studdert-Kennedy and Shankweiler (1970)[19] examine dichotic listening of CVC syllable pairs. The six stop consonants (b, d, g, p, t, k) are paired with the six vowels and a variation in the initial and final consonants are analyzed. REA is the strongest when the sound of the initial and final consonants differ and it is the weakest when solely the vowel is changed. Asbjornsen and Bryden (1996) state that "many researchers have chosen to use CV syllable pairs, usually consisting of the six stop consonants paired with the vowel \a\. Over the years, a large amount of data has been generated using such material."[20]
Selective Attention
In selective attention experiments, the participants may be asked to repeat aloud the content of the message they are listening to. This task is known as shadowing. As Colin Cherry (1953)[21] found, people do not recall the shadowed message well, suggesting that most of the processing necessary to shadow the attended message occurs in working memory and is not preserved in the long-term store. Performance on the unattended message is worse. Participants are generally able to report almost nothing about the content of the unattended message. In fact, a change from English to German in the unattended channel frequently goes unnoticed. However, participants are able to report that the unattended message is speech rather than non-verbal content. In addition to this, if the content of the unattended message contains certain information, such as the listener's name, then the unattended message is more likely to be noticed and remembered.[22] Also if the message contains sexual words then people usually notice them immediately.[23] This suggests that the unattended information is also undergoing analysis and keywords can divert attention to it. Dichotic Listening also relates to the third basic property of consciousness: Selectivity which is the capacity to include some objects but not others.[24]
Gender differences
Some data gathered from dichotic listening test experiments suggests that there is possibly a small-population sex difference in perceptual and auditory asymmetries and language laterality. According to Voyer (2011),[25] "Dichotic listening tasks produced homogenous effect sizes regardless of task type (verbal, non-verbal), reflecting a significant sex difference in the magnitude of laterality effects, with men obtaining larger laterality effects than women.[25] However, the authors discuss numerous limiting factors ranging from publication bias to small effect size. Furthermore, as discussed in "Attention, reliability, and validity of perceptual asymmetries in the fused dichotic words test,"[26] women reported more "intrusions" of words presented to the uncued ear than men when presented with exogenous cues in the Fused Dichotic Word Task which suggests two possibilities: 1) Women experience more difficulty paying attention to the cued word than men and/or 2) regardless of the cue, women spread their attention evenly as opposed to men who may possibly focus in more intently on exogenous cues.[25]
Dichotic listening on schizophrenia
A study conducted involving the dichotic listening test,[27] with emphasis on subtypes of schizophrenia (particularly paranoid and undifferentiated), demonstrated that Paranoid schizophrenia have the largest left hemisphere advantage - with undifferentiated schizophrenics (where psychotic symptoms are present but the criteria for paranoid, disorganized, or catatonic types have not been met) having the smallest. The application of the dichotic listening test helped to further the beliefs that preserved left hemisphere processing is a product of paranoid schizophrenia, and in contrast, that the left hemisphere's lack of activity is a symptom of undifferentiated schizophrenia. In 1994, M. F. Green tried to relate “the functional integration of the left hemisphere in hallucinating and nonhallucinating psychotic patients”[28] using a dichotic listening study. The study showed that auditory hallucinations are connected to a malfunction in the left hemisphere of the brain.
History
In the late 1960s and early 1970s Donald Shankweiler[29] and Michael Studdert-Kennedy[30] of Haskins Laboratories used a dichotic listening technique (presenting different nonsense syllables) to demonstrate the dissociation of phonetic (speech) and auditory (nonspeech) perception by finding that phonetic structure devoid of meaning is an integral part of language and is typically processed in the left cerebral hemisphere.[19][31][32] A dichotic listening performance advantage for one ear is interpreted as indicating a processing advantage in the contralateral hemisphere. In another example, Sidtis (1981)[33] found that healthy adults have a left-ear advantage on a dichotic pitch recognition experiment. He interpreted this result as indicating right-hemisphere dominance for pitch discrimination.
During the early 1970s, Tim Rand[34] demonstrated dichotic perception at Haskins Laboratories.[35] In his study, the first stimuli: formant (F1), was presented to one ear while the second and third stimuli:(F2) and (F3) formants, were presented to the opposite ear. F2 and F3 varied in low and high intensity. Ultimately, in comparison to the binaural condition, "peripheral masking is avoided when speech is heard dichotically."[35] This demonstration was originally known as "the Rand effect" but was later renamed "dichotic release from masking". The name for this demonstration continued to evolve and was finally named "dichotic perception" or "dichotic listening." Around the same time, Jim Cutting (1976),[36] an investigator at Haskins Laboratories, researched how listeners could correctly identify syllables when different components of the syllable were presented to different ears. The formants of vowel sounds and their relation are crucial in differentiating vowel sounds. Even though the listeners heard two separate signals with neither ear receiving a 'complete' vowel sound, they could still identify the syllable sounds.
References
- ↑ Ingram, John C. L. (2007). Neurolinguistics : an introduction to spoken language processing and its disorders (1. publ., 3. print. ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 381. ISBN 978-0-521-79640-8.
- ↑ Ingram, John C. L. (2007). Neurolinguistics : an introduction to spoken language processing and its disorders (1. publ., 3. print. ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-79640-8.
- ↑ Schacter, GILBERT, WEGNER, Daniel (2011). Psychology (1. publ., 3. print. ed.). Cambridge: WorthPublishers. p. 180. ISBN 978-1-429-24107-6.
- ↑ Johnson JP, Sommers RK, Weidner WE (March 1977). "Dichotic ear preference in aphasia". J Speech Hear Res 20 (1): 116–29. doi:10.1044/jshr.2001.116. PMID 846195.
- 1 2 Wexler BE, Halwes T (1983). "Increasing the power of dichotic methods: the fused rhymed words test". Neuropsychologia 21 (1): 59–66. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(83)90100-8. PMID 6843817.
- 1 2 Zatorre RJ (1989). "Perceptual asymmetry on the dichotic fused words test and cerebral speech lateralization determined by the carotid sodium amytal test". Neuropsychologia 27 (10): 1207–19. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(89)90033-X. PMID 2480551.
- ↑ Grimshaw GM, Kwasny KM, Covell E, Johnson RA (2003). "The dynamic nature of language lateralization: effects of lexical and prosodic factors". Neuropsychologia 41 (8): 1008–19. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00315-9. PMID 12667536.
- ↑ Hahn C, Neuhaus AH, Pogun S, et al. (July 2011). "Smoking reduces language lateralization: a dichotic listening study with control participants and schizophrenia patients". Brain Cogn 76 (2): 300–9. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.015. PMID 21524559.
- 1 2 Arciuli J (July 2011). "Manipulation of voice onset time during dichotic listening". Brain Cogn 76 (2): 233–8. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.01.007. PMID 21320740.
- ↑ Rimol LM, Eichele T, Hugdahl K (2006). "The effect of voice-onset-time on dichotic listening with consonant-vowel syllables". Neuropsychologia 44 (2): 191–6. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.006. PMID 16023155.
- ↑ Westerhausen R, Helland T, Ofte S, Hugdahl K (2010). "A longitudinal study of the effect of voicing on the dichotic listening ear advantage in boys and girls at age 5 to 8". Dev Neuropsychol 35 (6): 752–61. doi:10.1080/87565641.2010.508551. PMID 21038164.
- ↑ Andersson M, Llera JE, Rimol LM, Hugdahl K (September 2008). "Using dichotic listening to study bottom-up and top-down processing in children and adults". Child Neuropsychol 14 (5): 470–9. doi:10.1080/09297040701756925. PMID 18608228.
- ↑ Arciuli J, Rankine T, Monaghan P (May 2010). "Auditory discrimination of voice-onset time and its relationship with reading ability". Laterality 15 (3): 343–60. doi:10.1080/13576500902799671. PMID 19343572.
- 1 2 Hugdahl K, Bodner T, Weiss E, Benke T (March 2003). "Dichotic listening performance and frontal lobe function". Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 16 (1): 58–65. doi:10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00210-0. PMID 12589889.
- 1 2 Westerhausen R, Hugdahl K (July 2008). "The corpus callosum in dichotic listening studies of hemispheric asymmetry: a review of clinical and experimental evidence". Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32 (5): 1044–54. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.04.005. PMID 18499255.
- ↑ Kimura, Doreen (1961). "Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli.". Canadian Journal of Psychology 15 (3): 166–171. doi:10.1037/h0083219. ISSN 0008-4255.
- ↑ Ingram, John C. L. (2007). Neurolinguistics : an introduction to spoken language processing and its disorders (1. publ., 3. print. ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 115. ISBN 978-0-521-79640-8.
- ↑ Kimura, Doreen (1967). "Functional Asymmetry of the Brain in Dichotic Listening". Cortex 3 (2): 163–178. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(67)80010-8. ISSN 0010-9452.
- 1 2 Studdert-Kennedy, Michael; Shankweiler, Donald (1970). "Hemispheric Specialization for Speech Perception". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 48 (2B): 579–594. doi:10.1121/1.1912174. ISSN 0001-4966.
- ↑ Asbjornsen AE, Bryden MP (May 1996). "Biased attention and the fused dichotic words test". Neuropsychologia 34 (5): 407–11. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(95)00127-1. PMID 9148197.
- ↑ Cherry, E. Colin (1953). "Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and with Two Ears". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 25 (5): 975. doi:10.1121/1.1907229. ISSN 0001-4966.
- ↑ Moray, Neville (1959). "Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions" (PDF). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 11 (1): 56–60. doi:10.1080/17470215908416289. ISSN 0033-555X.
- ↑ Nielsen, Stevan L.; Sarason, Irwin G. (1981). "Emotion, personality, and selective attention.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 41 (5): 945–960. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.41.5.945. ISSN 0022-3514.
- ↑ Schacter, Daniel L (2010). Psychology. New York: Worth Publishers. ISBN 1-4292-3719-8. OCLC 696604625.
- 1 2 3 Voyer D (July 2011). "Sex differences in dichotic listening". Brain Cogn 76 (2): 245–55. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.02.001. PMID 21354684.
- ↑ Voyer, Daniel; Jennifer Ingram (2005). "Attention, reliability, and validity of perceptual asymmetries in the fused dichotic word test". Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain, and Cognition 6: 545–561. doi:10.1080/13576500442000292.
- ↑ Friedman MS, Bruder GE, Nestor PG, Stuart BK, Amador XF, Gorman JM (September 2001). "Perceptual asymmetries in schizophrenia: subtype differences in left hemisphere dominance for dichotic fused words" (PDF). Am J Psychiatry 158 (9): 1437–40. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.9.1437. PMID 11532728.
- ↑ Green MF, Hugdahl K, Mitchell S (March 1994). "Dichotic listening during auditory hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia". Am J Psychiatry 151 (3): 357–62. PMID 8109643.
- ↑ "Donald P. Shankweiler".
- ↑ "Michael Studdert-Kennedy".
- ↑ Studdert-Kennedy M., Shankweiler D., Schulman S. (1970). "Opposed effects of a delayed channel on perception of dichotically and monotically presented CV syllables". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 48: 599–602. doi:10.1121/1.1912179.
- ↑ Studdert-Kennedy M., Shankweiler D., Pisoni D. (1972). "Auditory and phonetic processes in speech perception: Evidence from a dichotic study". Journal of Cognitive Psychology 2: 455–466.
- ↑ Sidtis, John J. (1981). "The complex tone test: Implications for the assessment of auditory laterality effects". Neuropsychologia 19 (1): 103–112. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(81)90050-6. ISSN 0028-3932.
- ↑ "Rand, T. C. (1974)". Haskins Laboratories Publications-R.
- 1 2 Rand T. C. (1974). "Dichotic release from masking for speech". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 55 (3): 678–680. doi:10.1121/1.1914584. PMID 4819869.
- ↑ Cutting, James E. (1976). "Auditory and linguistic processes in speech perception: Inferences from six fusions in dichotic listening." (PDF). Psychological Review 83 (2): 114–140. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.83.2.114. ISSN 0033-295X.
Further reading
- Hugdahl, Kenneth (1988). Handbook of dichotic listening: theory, methods and research. New York: Wiley. ISBN 0-471-91267-0. OCLC 18350007.