Expected satiety

The term expected satiety refers to the satiety (relief from hunger) that is expected from a particular food. It is closely associated with 'expected satiation' which refers to the immediate fullness (post meal) that a food is expected to generate.

Scientists have discovered that foods differ considerably in their expected satiety. One estimate suggests that there may be a six-fold difference in commonly consumed foods (in the UK), when they are compared calorie for calorie.[1] This range of variation is important because expected satiety is thought to be a good predictor of food choice and an excellent predictor of self-selected portion sizes.[2] Specifically, foods that have high expected satiety and high expected satiation tend to be selected in smaller portions (fewer calories). Therefore, they may be especially suited to diets that are designed to reduce energy intake.

Some researchers also suggest that expected satiety is an important mediator of energy intake.[3][4] They argue that within-meal events (immediate post-ingestive feedback, e.g., gastric stretch) play a relatively minor role and that meal size is largely determined by decisions about portion size, before a meal begins. Consistent with this proposition, observational studies show that 'plate cleaning' is extremely common,[5] that humans tend to plan their meal size in advance, and that ad libitum eating is relatively rare.[6][7]

Measurement

Selecting a portion in a measure of expected satiety

Early approaches relied on rating scales.[8][9] More recently, techniques have been developed that quantify expectations very precisely by comparing foods directly on a calorie-for-calorie basis. The first of these used a classical psychophysical approach based on a 'method of constant stimuli'.[10] Participants are shown a fixed 'standard' portion of food and this is compared against a different 'comparison' food. Over a series of trials the size of the comparison food is manipulated and participants are asked to pick the food that is expected to deliver greater satiety. At the end of the task a measure of 'expected satiety' is calculated. This relates to the number of calories of the comparison food that would be expected to deliver the same satiety as the fixed standard. A conceptually similar alternative is to use a 'method of adjustment'. Participants are shown a picture of a standard food next to a picture of a comparison food. Using specialist software, participants change the size of the comparison portion using keyboard responses.[11] Pictures are loaded with sufficient speed that the change in the comparison becomes 'animated.' Participants are told to match the comparison food until both are expected to deliver the same satiety. If the same standard is used then the expected satiety of different foods can be quantified and compared directly.

Determinants

Expectations about the post-ingestive effects of a food are learned over time.[12] In particular, it would appear that the expected satiety and expected satiation of foods increases as they become familiar.[13][14][15]

Expectations are also thought to be governed by the orosensory characteristics of food. Even subtle changes to the flavor or texture of food can have a marked effect.[16][17] Expected satiation may be higher in foods that have a higher protein content, and in those that require more chewing and that are eaten slowly.[18][19] Remarkably, it also appears that the expected satiety and expected satiation of foods is influenced by their perceived weight.[20]

Effect on appetite

The effects of expected satiety and expected satiation appear to extend beyond meal planning. Several studies show that these expectations also influence the hunger and fullness that is experienced after a meal has been consumed.[21] Product labelling and branding is likely to modify expected satiety.[22] Therefore, this kind of information has the potential to influence appetite directly. Together, these observations are consistent with emerging evidence that implicates hippocampal-dependent memory mechanisms in behavioural responses to food.[23][24][25]

Notes

Sometimes expected satiety is otherwise referred to as 'satiety expectations'.[26]

References

  1. Brunstrom, J.M. (2008). "Measuring 'expected satiety' in a range of common foods using a method of constant stimuli.". Appetite 51: 604–614. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2008.04.017. PMID 18547677.
  2. Brunstrom, J.M. (2012). "How many calories are on our plate? Expected fullness, not liking, determines meal-size selection". Obesity 17 (10): 1884–1890. doi:10.1038/oby.2009.201. PMID 19543204.
  3. Brunstrom, J.M. (2014). "Mind over platter: pre-meal planning and the control of meal size in humans". International Journal of Obesity 38: S9–12. doi:10.1038/ijo.2014.83.
  4. Forde, C.G. (2015). "Expected Satiety: Application to weight management and understanding energy selection in humans". Current Obesity Reports 4: 131–140. doi:10.1007/s13679-015-0144-0.
  5. Wansink, B. (2014). "The clean plate club: about 92% of self-served food is eaten". International Journal of Obesity 39: 371–374. doi:10.1038/ijo.2014.104.
  6. Wilkinson, L.L. (2012). "Computer-based assessments of expected satiety predict behavioural measures of portion-size selection and food intake". Appetite 59: 933–938. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.007.
  7. Fay, S. (2011). "What determines real-world meal size? Evidence for pre-meal planning". Appetite 56: 284–289. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.006. PMID 21232568.
  8. Green, S.M. (1996). "Subjective and objective indices of the satiating effect of foods. Can people predict how filling a food will be?". European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 50: 798–806.
  9. de Graaf, C. (1992). "Beliefs about the satiating effect of bread with spread varying in macronutrient content". Appetite 18: 121–128. doi:10.1016/0195-6663(92)90189-d.
  10. Brunstrom, J.M. (2008). "Measuring 'expected satiety' in a range of common foods using a method of constant stimuli". Appetite 51: 604–614. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2008.04.017. PMID 18547677.
  11. Brunstrom, J.M. (2009). "How many calories are on our plate? Expected fullness, not liking, determines meal-size selection". Obesity 17: 1884–1890. doi:10.1038/oby.2009.201. PMID 19543204.
  12. Brunstrom, J.M. (2009). "Conditioning expectations about the satiating quality of food". Appetite 52 (3): 780–783.
  13. Irvine, M. (2012). "Increased familiarity with eating a food to fullness underlies increased expected satiety". Appetite 61 (13-18): 13–18. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.10.011.
  14. Hardman, C. (2011). "Children's familiarity with snack foods changes expectations about fullness". American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 94 (5): 1196–201. doi:10.3945/ajcn.111.016873.
  15. Brunstrom, J.M. (2010). "Familiarity changes expectations about fullness". Appetite 54 (3): 587–90. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2010.01.015.
  16. Hogenkamp, P.S. (2012). "Expected satiation after repeated consumption of low- or high-energy-dense soup". British Journal of Nutrition 108 (1): 182–190. doi:10.1017/s0007114511005344.
  17. McCrickerd, K. (2012). "Subtle changes in the flavour and texture of a drink enhance expectations of satiety". Flavour Sci Recent Dev. 1 (20): 1–11. doi:10.1186/2044-7248-1-20.
  18. Forde, C.G. (2013). "Oral processing characteristics of solid savoury meal components, and relationship with food composition, sensory attributes and expected satiation". Appetite 60 (1): 208–219. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.015.
  19. Ferriday, D. (2013). "Exploring relationships between expected satiation, eating topography and actual satiety across a range of meals". Appetite 71 (1): 474. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2013.06.021.
  20. Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2012). "The weight of the container influences expected satiety, perceived density, and subsequent expected fullness". Appetite 58: 559–562. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.12.021.
  21. Brunstrom, J.M. (2011). "'Expected satiety' changes hunger and fullness in the inter-meal interval". Appetite 56 (2): 310–5. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.002.
  22. Fay, S.H. (2011). "Product labelling can confer sustained increases in expected and actual satiety". Appetite 57 (2): 557. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.05.069.
  23. Brunstrom, J.M. (2012). "Episodic memory and appetite regulation in humans". PLOS ONE 7 (12): e50707. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0050707.
  24. Higgs, S. (2002). "Memory for recent eating and its influence on subsequent food intake". Appetite 39 (2): 159–66. doi:10.1006/appe.2002.0500.
  25. Kanoski, S.E. (2011). "Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: Links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity". Physiology and Behavior 103 (1): 59–68. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.12.003. PMC 3056912. PMID 21167850.
  26. McCrickerd, K. (2012). "Subtle changes in the flavour and texture of a drink enhance expectations of satiety". Flavour 1: 1–20. doi:10.1186/2044-7248-1-20.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Monday, January 25, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.