First impression (psychology)
In psychology, a first impression is the event when one person first encounters another person and forms a mental image of that person. Impression accuracy varies depending on the observer and the target (person, object, scene, etc.) being observed.[1][2] First impressions are based on a wide range of characteristics: age, race, culture, language, gender, physical appearance, accent, posture, voice, number of people present, and time allowed to process.[1][3][4][5][6][7][8] The first impressions individuals give to others could greatly influence how they are treated and viewed in many contexts of everyday life.[9][10]
Speed and accuracy
It takes just one-tenth of a second for us to judge someone and make a first impression.[11] Research finds that the more time participants are afforded to form the impression, the more confidence in impressions they report.[11][12] Not only are people quick to form first impressions, they are also fairly accurate when the target presents him or herself genuinely. People are generally not good at perceiving feigned emotions or detecting lies.[2] Research participants who reported forming accurate impressions of specific targets did tend to have more accurate perceptions of specific targets that aligned with others' reports of the target.[4] Individuals are also fairly reliable at understanding the first impression that he/she will project to others.[13]
The rate at which different qualities are detected in first impressions may be linked to what has been important to survival from an evolutionary perspective.[11] For example, trustworthiness and attractiveness were the two traits most quickly detected and evaluated in a study of human faces.[11][12] People are fairly good at assessing personality traits of others in general, but there appears to be a difference in first impression judgments between older and younger adults. Older adults judged young adult target photos as healthier, more trustworthy, and less hostile, but more aggressive, than younger adults did of the same photos.[14] Older adults could have a lower response to negative cues due to a slower processing speed, causing them to see facial features on young adults as more positive than younger adults do.[14]
Number of observers
One's first impressions are affected by whether he or she is alone or with any number of people.[5] Joint experiences are more globally processed (see global precedence for more on processing), as in collectivist cultures. Global processing emphasizes first impressions more because the collective first impression tends to remain stable over time. Solo experiences tend to facilitate local processing, causing the viewer to take a more critical look at the target. Thus, individuals are more likely to have negative first impressions than groups of two or more viewers of the same target. At the same time, individuals are more likely to experience an upward trend over the course of a series of impressions, e.g. individual viewers will like the final episode of a TV season more than the first even if it is really the same quality.[5]
When viewing pieces of art in an experiment, participants in a solo context rated art in an improving sequence significantly higher than when the targets are presented in a declining sequence.[5] When viewing the art in a joint context, participants evaluated the first and last pieces similarly in both kinds of sequence. Simply priming viewers to feel like they were in solo or joint contexts or to process analytically or holistically was enough to produce the same viewing effects.[5]
Cultural influences
Individualism versus collectivism
Similar to the number of viewers present, collectivism versus individualism can influence impression formation.[3] Collectivists are at ease as long as their impressions are largely in alignment with the larger group's impressions. When a collectivist wants to change his/her impression, he/she may be compelled to change the views of all group members.[15] However, this could be challenging for collectivists, who tend to be less confrontational than individualists. Individualists are willing change their own views at will and are generally more comfortable with uncertainty, which makes them naturally more willing to change their impressions.[15]
Influence of media richness
Research is mixed on whether national culture mediates the relationship between media richness and bias in impression formation. Some studies that manipulated media richness have found that information presented in text form yields similar impressions (measured by reported appraisal scores) among cultures,[15] while other studies found that richer forms of information such as videos reduce cross-cultural bias more effectively.[16] The latter findings support Media Richness Theory.[16]
Accents and speech
Accents and unique speech patterns can influence how people are perceived by those to whom they are speaking. For example, when hypothetically interviewing an applicant with a Midwestern U.S. accent, Colombian accent, or French accent, Midwestern U.S. participants evaluated the U.S. accent as significantly more positive than the applicant with the French accent due to perceived similarity to themselves.[17] The evaluation of the applicant with the Colombian accent did not, however, differ significantly from the other two. First impressions can be heavily influenced by a similarity-attraction hypothesis where others are immediately put into “similar" or “dissimilar” categories from the viewer and judged accordingly.[17]
Physical characteristics and personality
Although populations from different cultures can be quick to view others as dissimilar, there are several first impression characteristics that are universal across cultures.[3] When comparing trait impressions of faces among U.S. and the culturally isolated Tsimane' people of Bolivia, there was between-culture agreement when ascribing certain physical features to descriptive traits such as attractiveness, intelligence, health, and warmth.[18] Both cultures also show a strong attractiveness halo when forming impressions, meaning that those seen as attractive were also rated as more competent, sociable, intelligent, and healthy.[18]
Physical appearance
Faces and features
Physical appearance gives us clear clues as to a person's personality without him/her ever having to speak or move.[3][6][7][19] Women tend to be better than men at judging nonverbal behavior.[3] After viewing pictures of people in a neutral position and in a self-chosen posed position, observers were accurate at judging the target's levels of extraversion, emotional stability, openness, self-esteem, and religiosity.[7] The combined impression of physical characteristics, body posture, facial expression, and clothing choices lets observers form accurate images of a target's personality, so long as the person observed is presenting themselves genuinely.[7][2] However, there is some conflicting data in this field. Other evidence suggests that people sometimes rely too much on appearance cues over actual information.[20][21] When provided with descriptive information about a target, participants still rely on physical appearance cues when making judgments about others' personalities and capabilities. Participants struggle to look past physical appearance cues even when they know information contrary to their initial judgment.[20] Physical cues are also used to make judgments about political candidates based on extremely brief exposures to their pictures.[21] Perceived competence level of a candidate measured from first impressions of facial features can directly predict voting results.[21]
The “beautiful is good” effect is a very present phenomenon when dealing with first impressions of others.[3] Targets who are attractive are rated more positively and as possessing more unique characteristics than those who are unattractive.[22] Beauty is also found to be somewhat subjective so that even targets who are not universally attractive can receive the benefit of this effect if the observer is attracted to them.[22]
In a 2014 study Hartley and colleagues at the University of York reported that impressions of the traits of approachability, youthfulness/attractiveness and dominance can be formed in as little as 100 milliseconds, from measurable characteristics such as the shape of and the spacing around the eyes, nose and mouth.[23] it was found that first impressions of social traits, such as trustworthiness or dominance, are reliably perceived in faces. Physical facial features were objectively measured from feature positions and colours. A neural network was the used to model factor the dimensions of approachability, youthful-attractiveness and dominance. 58% of the variance in raters’ impressions was accounted for by a linear model.[24]
Apparel and cosmetics
Cosmetic use is also an important cue for forming impressions, particularly of women. Those wearing heavy makeup are seen as significantly more feminine than those wearing moderate makeup or no makeup and those wearing heavy or moderate makeup are seen as more attractive than those wearing no makeup.[19] While a woman wearing no makeup is perceived as being more moral than the other two conditions, there is no difference between experimental conditions when judging personality or personal temperament.[19]
First impression formation can be influenced by the use of cognitive short hands such as stereotypes and representative heuristics.[3] When asked to rate the socioeconomic status (SES) and degree of interest in friendship with African American and Caucasian female models wearing either a K-Mart, Abercrombie & Fitch, or non-logoed sweatshirt, Caucasian models were rated more favorably than the African American models.[25] Abercrombie & Fitch wearers were rated as higher SES than the other sweatshirts. Interestingly, participants wanted to be friends with the Caucasian model most when she was wearing a plain sweatshirt and the African American model most when she was wearing either the plain or K-Mart sweatshirt. It is unclear why the plain sweatshirt was most associated with friendship, but the general results suggest that mismatching class and race reduced the model's friendship appeal.[25]
Specific contexts
Online
Online profiles and communication channels such as email provide fewer cues than in-person interactions, which makes targets more difficult to understand.[3] When research participants were asked to evaluate a person's facial attractiveness and perceived ambition based on an online dating profile, amount of time permitted for processing and reporting an evaluation of the target produced a difference in impression formation.[26] Spontaneous evaluations relied on physical attractiveness almost exclusively, whereas deliberate evaluations weighed both types of information. Although deliberate evaluations used the information provided on both physical attractiveness and ambition of each target, the particular impact of each kind of information appeared to depend on the consistency between the two. A significant effect of attractiveness on deliberate evaluations was found only when perceived ambition was consistent with the perceived level of attractiveness.[26] The consistency found in profiles seemed to particularly influence deliberate evaluations.
In a study of online impressions, participants who were socially expressive and disclosed a lot about themselves both on their webpages and in person were better liked than those who were less open. Social expressivity includes liveliness in voice, smiling, etc.[27]
Dating and sexuality
Upon seeing photographs of straight, gay, and bisexual people, participants correctly identified gay versus straight males and females at above-chance levels based solely on seeing a picture of their face, however, bisexual targets were only identified at chance.[6] The data suggest that people do not differentiate between bisexual targets and gay targets, but do perceive a difference between straight targets and those who identify as either bisexual or gay.
The more time participants are allowed to make some judgment about a person, the more they will weigh information beyond physical appearance. Specific manipulations include identifying men as gay versus straight[28] and people as trustworthy or not.[4][12] In a study of the interaction between ratings of people in speed dating and the form of media used to present them, impression accuracy in a speed dating task was not significantly different when a potential date was presented in person versus in a video. However, impressions of dates made via video were to be much more negative than those made in person. An additional study that looked at characterization of a romantic partner suggested that people are more likely to rely on “gut reactions” when meeting in person, but there isn't sufficient information for this kind of evaluation when viewing someone online.
Professional
Non-verbal behaviors are particularly important to forming first impressions when meeting a business acquaintance.[29] Specifically, components of social expressivity, such as smiling, eyebrow position, emotional expression, and eye contact are emphasized.[2][27][29] Straightening one's posture, leaning in slightly, and giving a firm handshake promotes favorable impression formation in the American business context.[29] Other impression management tactics in the business world include researching the organization and interviewers beforehand, preparing specific questions for the interviewer, showing confidence, and dressing appropriately.[30]
A qualitative review of previous literature looking at self-report data suggests that men and women use impression management tactics in the corporate world that are consistent with stereotypical gender roles when presenting themselves to others.[9] This research proposes that women are put in a double bind where those who portray themselves as more communal and submissive are overlooked for leadership positions and women who try to utilize male tactics (such as being more aggressive) receive negative consequences for violating normative gender roles.[9] To change this dynamic the authors suggest that managerial positions should be re-advertised to highlight the feminine qualities needed for a position and staff training should involve a segment accentuating gender issues in the office to make everyone aware of possible discrimination.[9]
Data collected from interviews with physicians distinguishes between first impressions and intuition and contributes to understanding the occurrence of gut feelings in the medical field.[31] Gut feelings go beyond first impressions: Physicians expressed feeling doubtful about their initial impressions as they gathered more data from their patients. More experienced physicians reported more instances of gut feelings than those less experienced, but the quality of the intuition was related to the quality of feedback received during the data collection process in general. Emotional engagement enhanced learning just as it does in first impressions.[31]
Evolutionary basis
An instantaneous evaluation about intentions of an approaching person or subject based upon visual clues might have very strong connection with initial stages of biological evolution. The reason why it happens so quickly inside our brains has to do with both risks and benefits subjected with any person or subject that is being encountered at very first time. The latency time of an entire evaluation process suggests a crucial adaption done by pre-historic creatures based upon their earlier experiences of encounters with strangers. Lesser time the process of identification takes, better are the chances of survival.
According to one hypothesis presented by Mr. Sachchidanand Swami (an independent Human Behavior and Nonverbal Communication Researcher from India), an instantaneous evaluation based upon visual clues obtained from any person or subject might have begun with evolution of vision itself. Our remote ancestors living inside pre-historic oceans with their primordial eyesight and small sized brains might have evolved this process. This 'seeing is believing' neurological reflex might have primarily evolved to identify predatory and non-predatory creatures as quickly as possible.
According to him, evaluating intentions of any person or subject based upon its visuals or an external appearance might ultimately have enforced trustworthiness, which is a crucial factor for an interpersonal relationship. Draft of this hypothesis was conveyed by him through an email to more than 100 scientists and researchers from many renowned universities, research laboratories and institutions. This hypothesis is still to be testified by scientific community.
Neuroscience
First impressions are formed within milliseconds of seeing a target. When intentionally forming a first impression, encoding relies on the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC).[32] Readings from fMRIs of research participants show that processing of diagnostic information (e.g. distinguishing features) engaged the dmPFC more than processing neutral information.[32]
Participants generally formed more negative impressions of the faces that showed a negative emotion compared to neutral faces.[33] Results suggest that the dmPFC and amygdala together play a large role in negative impression formation. When forming immediate impressions based on emotion, the stimulus can bypass the neo-cortex by way of the “amygdala hijack.”[34]
Familiarity
Research indicates that people are efficient evaluators when forming impressions based on existing biases.[35] The posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), amygdala, and the thalamus sort relevant versus irrelevant information according to these biases. The dmPFC is also involved in the impression formation process, especially with person-descriptive information.[35]
FMRI results show activation of the fusiform cortex, posterior cingulate gyrus, and amygdala when individuals are asked to identify previously seen faces that were encoded as either “friends” or “foes.”[36] Additionally, the caudate and anterior cingulate cortex are more activated when looking at faces of “foes” versus “friends."[36] This research suggests that quick first impressions of hostility or support from unknown people can lead to long-term effects on memory that will later be associated with that person.
Alcohol and impressions
Alcohol consumption and belief of consumption influenced emotion detection in ten second clips.[37] Participants who thought they had consumed an alcoholic beverage rated one facial expression (approximately 3% of the facial expressions they saw) more in each clip as happy compared to the control group. Thus, impression formation may be affected by even the perception of alcohol consumption.[37]
Cross-cultural differences
There appears to be cross-cultural similarities in brain responses to first impression formations. In a mock election both American and Japanese individuals voted for the candidate that elicited a stronger response in their bilateral amygdala than those who did not, regardless of the candidate's culture.[38] Individuals also showed a stronger response to cultural outgroup faces than cultural ingroup faces because the amygdala is presumably more sensitive to novel stimuli.[38] However, this finding was unrelated to actual voting decisions.[38]
Stability of first impressions
Once formed, first impressions tend to be stable. A review of the literature on the accuracy and impact of first impressions on rater-based assessments found that raters' first impressions are highly correlated with later scores, but it is unclear exactly why.[10] One study tested stability by asking participants to form impressions people based purely on photographs. Participants' opinions of the people in photographs did not significantly differ after interacting with that person a month later.[28] One potential reason for this stability is that one's first impressions could serve as a guide for his/her next steps, such as what questions are asked and how raters go about scoring. More research needs to be done on the stability of first impressions to fully understand how first impressions guide subsequent treatment, self-fulfilling prophecies, and the halo effect.[3] Assessment tools can influence impressions too, for example if a question provides only a dichotomous "yes" or "no" response or if a rater uses a scale (ratio). Although this study was conducted with the intention of improving rating methods in medical education, the literature review was sufficiently broad enough to generalize.[28]
See also
References
- 1 2 Mackie, Eliot R. Smith, Diane M. (2007). Social psychology (3rd ed.). Hove: Psychology Press. ISBN 1841694096. Retrieved 8 May 2014.
- 1 2 3 4 Flora, Carlin. "The First Impression". Psychology Today. Archived from the original on 2011-02-20. Retrieved 2011-02-20.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Aronson, Elliot, Robin M. Akert, Timothy D. Wilson (2007). Social psychology (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice-Hall. ISBN 0132382458. Retrieved 05/08/14. Check date values in:
|access-date=
(help) - 1 2 3 Biesanz, J.C.; Human, L. J.; Paquin, A.; Chan, M.; Parisotto, K. L.; Sarracino, J.; Gillis, R. L. (2011). "Do we know when our impressions of others are valid? evidence for realistic accuracy awareness in first impressions of personality". Social Psychological and Personality Science 2 (5). doi:10.1177/1948550610397211.
- 1 2 3 4 5 Bhargrave, R; Montgomery, N.V. "The social context of temporal sequences: Why first impressions shape shared experiences". Journal of Consumer Research 40 (3): 501–517. doi:10.1086/671053.
- 1 2 3 Ding, Jonathan Y. C.; Rule, Nicholas O. (12 January 2012). "Gay, Straight, or Somewhere in Between: Accuracy and Bias in the Perception of Bisexual Faces". Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 36 (2): 165–176. doi:10.1007/s10919-011-0129-y.
- 1 2 3 4 Naumann, L. P.; Vazire, S.; Rentfrow, P. J.; Gosling, S. D. (17 September 2009). "Personality Judgments Based on Physical Appearance". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 35 (12): 1661–1671. doi:10.1177/0146167209346309.
- ↑ Trudeau, M. (5 May 2014). "You Had Me At Hello: The Science Behind First Impressions". NPR. Retrieved 6 May 2014.
- 1 2 3 4 Guadagno, Rosanna E.; Cialdini, Robert B. (21 March 2007). "Gender Differences in Impression Management in Organizations: A Qualitative Review". Sex Roles 56 (7-8): 483–494. doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9187-3.
- 1 2 Wood, T.J. (August 2014). "Exploring the role of first impressions in rater-based assessments". Advances in Health Sciences Education 19 (3): 409–427. doi:10.1007/s10459-013-9453-9.
- 1 2 3 4 Willis, J.; Todorov, A. (2006). "First impressions: Making up your mind after 100 ms exposure to a face" (PDF). Psychological Science (17): 592–598. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x. Retrieved 17 May 2014.
- 1 2 3 Wargo, E (2006). "How many seconds to a first impression?". The Observer 19.
- ↑ Carlson, E. N.; Furr, R. M.; Vazire, S. (1 January 2010). "Do We Know the First Impressions We Make? Evidence for Idiographic Meta-Accuracy and Calibration of First Impressions". Social Psychological and Personality Science 1 (1): 94–98. doi:10.1177/1948550609356028.
- 1 2 Zebrowitz, Leslie A.; Franklin, Robert G.; Hillman, Suzanne; Boc, Henry. "Older and younger adults' first impressions from faces: Similar in agreement but different in positivity.". Psychology and Aging 28 (1): 202–212. doi:10.1037/a0030927.
- 1 2 3 Fang, X.; Rajkumar, T. M. (2013). "The Role of National Culture and Multimedia on First Impression Bias Reduction: An Experimental Study in US and China". IEEE Transactions On Professional Communication 56 (4): 354–371. doi:10.1109/TPC.2013.2251503.
- 1 2 Lim, K. H.; Benbasat, I.; Ward, L. M. (2000). "The role of multimedia in changing first impression bias". Information Systems Research 11 (2): 115–136. doi:10.1287/isre.11.2.115.11776.
- 1 2 Deprez-Sims, Anne-Sophie; Morris, Scott B. "Accents in the workplace: Their effects during a job interview". International Journal of Psychology 45 (6): 417–426. doi:10.1080/00207594.2010.499950.
- 1 2 Zebrowitz, L. A.; Wang, R.; Bronstad, P. M.; Eisenberg, D.; Undurraga, E.; Reyes-Garcia, V.; Godoy, R. (19 December 2011). "First Impressions From Faces Among U.S. and Culturally Isolated Tsimane' People in the Bolivian Rainforest". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43 (1): 119–134. doi:10.1177/0022022111411386.
- 1 2 3 Workman, J. E.; Johnson, K. K. P. (1 September 1991). "The Role of Cosmetics in Impression Formation". Clothing and Textiles Research Journal 10 (1): 63–67. doi:10.1177/0887302X9101000109.
- 1 2 Olivola, Christopher Y.; Todorov, Alexander. "Fooled by first impressions? Reexamining the diagnostic value of appearance-based inferences". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46 (2): 315–324. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.12.002.
- 1 2 3 Todorov, A. (10 June 2005). "Inferences of Competence from Faces Predict Election Outcomes". Science 308 (5728): 1623–1626. doi:10.1126/science.1110589.
- 1 2 Lorenzo, G. L.; Biesanz, J. C.; Human, L. J. (4 November 2010). "What Is Beautiful Is Good and More Accurately Understood: Physical Attractiveness and Accuracy in First Impressions of Personality". Psychological Science 21 (12): 1777–1782. doi:10.1177/0956797610388048.
- ↑ "First impressions connected to facial features - CNN.com". Edition.cnn.com. Retrieved 1 August 2014.
- ↑ "Modeling first impressions from highly variable facial images". Pnas.org. 2014-07-28. Retrieved 1 August 2014.
- 1 2 McDermott, L.; Pettijohn, T. (2011). "The influence of clothing fashion and race on the perceived socioeconomic status and person perception of college students". Psychology & Society 4: 64–75.
- 1 2 Sritharan, R.; Heilpern, K.; Wilbur, C. J.; Gawronski, B. (2010). "I think I like you: Spontaneous and deliberate evaluations of potential romantic partners in an online dating context". European Journal of Social Psychology 40: 1062–1077. doi:10.1002/ejsp.703.
- 1 2 Weisbuch, M.; Ivcevic, Z.; Ambady, N. (2009). "On being liked on the web and in the 'real world': Consistency in first impressions across personal web pages and spontaneous behavior". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45 (3): 573–576. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.12.009.
- 1 2 3 Wood, J. "The Power of a First Impression". Psych Central. Retrieved 6 May 2014.
- 1 2 3 Goman, Carol Kinsey (2008). The nonverbal advantage : secrets and science of body language at work ([Online-Ausg.]. ed.). San Francisco, Calif.: Berrett-Koehler Publishers. ISBN 1576754928.
- ↑ Rowh, Mark. "First Impressions Count". American Psychological Association. Retrieved 18 April 2014.
- 1 2 Woolley, A.; Kostopoulou, O. (2013). "Clinical intuition in family medicine: More than first impressions". Annals of Family Medicine 11: 60–66. doi:10.1370/afm.1433.
- 1 2 Gilron, R.; Gutchess, A. H. (2012). "Remembering first impressions: Effects of intentionality and diagnosticity on subsequent memory". Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience 12 (1): 85–98. doi:10.3758/s13415-011-0074-6.
- ↑ Iidaka, T.; Harada, T.; Sadato, N. (2011). "Forming a negative impression of another person correlates with activation in medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala". Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosciene 6 (4): 516–525. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq072.
- ↑ Goleman, Daniel (1999). Working with Emotional Intelligence. p. 87.
- 1 2 Schiller, D.; Freeman, J. B.; Mitchell, J. P.; Uleman, J. S.; Phelps, E. A. (2009). "A neural mechanism of first impressions". Nature Neuroscience 12: 508–514. doi:10.1038/nn.2278.
- 1 2 Vrticka, Pascal; Andersson, Frédéric; Sander, David; Vuilleumier, Patrik. "Memory for friends or foes: The social context of past encounters with faces modulates their subsequent neural traces in the brain". Social Neuroscience 4 (5): 384–401. doi:10.1080/17470910902941793.
- 1 2 Walter; Walter, N., Mutic, S., Markett, S., Montag, C., Klein, A., Reuter, M. (2011). "The Influence of Alcohol Intake and Alcohol Expectations on the Recognition of Emotions" 46: 680–685. doi:10.1093/alcalc/agr082. Cite uses deprecated parameter
|coauthors=
(help) - 1 2 3 Rule, N. O.; Freeman, J. B.; Moran, J. M.; Gabrieli, J. D. E.; Adams, R. B.; Ambady, N. (5 December 2009). "Voting behavior is reflected in amygdala response across cultures". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2-3): 349–355. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp046.