Gender and emotional expression

It can be tempting to generalize when considering the differences between men, women, and the various ways in which they express their emotions; men and women from various cultures have been shown to accept the stereotype that women are more emotional than men.[1] Typically, this perspective on men is only held in cases of discrete emotions and their corresponding expressions such as happiness, fear, disgust, or sadness. When men experience pride or anger, however, most people are much less likely to assume this stereotype. Leaving gender stereotypes unchallenged would have various potential consequences. The belief that women are more emotional than men questions our understanding of emotional reactivity and regulation systems. Furthermore, this assumption can have profound impacts on the way we interact with one another and our society at large. For these reasons, an emerging division of modern psychology research is dedicated to the critical examination of these stereotypes [2]

Many researchers have concluded that the observed emotional differences between men and women primarily stem from socialized gender roles, rather than biology. Thus, the range of emotional expression that certain populations experience is based, in large part, on their culture’s expectations for femininity and masculinity [3]

Major Theories

Many psychologists reject the notion that men actually experience emotions less frequently than do women. Instead, researchers have suggested that men exhibit restrictive emotionality. Restrictive emotionality refers to a tendency to inhibit the expression of certain emotions and an unwillingness to self-disclose intimate feelings.[4] Men’s restrictive emotionality has been shown to influence health, emotional appraisal and overall identity. Furthermore, tendencies toward restrictive emotionality are correlated with an increased risk of certain anxiety disorders. [4] A substantial amount of research has suggested that women do, on average, express emotions more frequently than men.[1] In the Handbook of Emotions, Leslie R. Brody and Judith A. Hall report that This difference in emotional expression starts at a young age, as early as 4 and 6 years old, as girls begin to express more sadness and anxiety than their male counterparts.[5][5] Brody and Hall (2008) report that women generally smile, laugh, nod and use hand gestures more than men do.[5] The only known exception to this rule is that men more frequently express anger. However, all of these effects are not commonly observed until after preschool, suggesting that these differences might be the result of certain socialization processes.[4] Women are also more accurate expressers of emotion, when “posing deliberately and when observed unobstrusively.” [5] This increased expressiveness in emotional expression is consistent across cultures – women report more intense emotional experiences and more overt emotional expressions across 37 cultures [6] It has been found that men and women more accurately display gender-stereotypic expressions– men more accurately express anger, contempt and happiness, while women were more accurately express fear and happiness.[5] Other studies have shown that women show higher levels of expression accuracy and judgement of nonverbal emotional cues than men overall.[5] However, these patterns are not consistent across cultures, again suggesting that socialization influences the observed gender differences in emotional expression.[5] For example, in Japan, research has suggested that women convey anger and contempt better than men do.[5]

Major empirical findings

Research has shown that culture and context-specific gender roles have a stronger influence on emotional expression than do biological factors. “Empirical evidence suggests that girls are socialized to be emotional, nonaggressive, nurturing, and obedient, whereas boys are socialized to be unemotional, aggressive, achievement oriented, and self-reliant. Peers continue this process as children develop and mature in effect constraining how, where, why, and with whom certain emotions are expressed”.[7] One study suggests that people tend to exhibit more intense negative facial expressions in solitary conditions, and smile more when others are present. In this experiment, men and women did not differ in their anger expression in non-social conditions. However, women were more likely to express their anger in the solitary condition as opposed to the social condition. Men, on the other hand, seemed to be less concerned with appearing positive to others; they showed no difference in their expression of anger based on whether or not others were present.[8] In another recent study, Coats and Feldman found that women who were more accurate expressors of happiness were judged as more popular, while men who were more accurate expressors of anger were judged as more popular.[9] This suggests that there are negative consequences for people who are less accurate expressors of gender-stereotypical emotions.

These consequences also extend to judging others’ emotions. Studies have shown that there are negative social consequences for children who are deficient at judging gender-stereotypic nonverbal cues – angry nonverbal cues for boys and happy, sad, and fearful nonverbal cues for girls. Communication of emotion involves both detection and expression of emotions or moods. The ability to detect non-verbal cues leads to successful communication of emotions. In computer-mediated communication (CMC), the absence of body language and visibility restricts one’s ability to correctly recognize other’s emotions. For this reason, emoticons are widely used in online communication to replace non-verbal behaviors that emphasize or clarify one’s feelings. Surprisingly, there is no static gender difference in the use of emoticons. In some studies, both men and women display an increase in emoticon use in the context of a mixed-gender group chat. Others show that men use more emoticons when interacting with women, while women show no change when interacting with men.[8]

Nature versus Nurture

The social-developmental hypothesis is one of the major arguments for the impact of nurture on emotional expression. The social-developmental theory explains gender differences in emotion expression through emphasizing “children’s active role in their development of gendered behavior” through learning by watching adults or through interactions with their parents and peers (Chaplin & Aldao, 2012).[10] This hypothesis points to the fact that infants are not born with the same differences in emotional expression and gender differences generally grow more pronounced as children age. In a 2012 meta-analysis conducted by Tara Chaplin and Amelia Aldao, researchers reviewed gender differences in emotion expression from the infancy period through adolescence in order to determine the impact of development and age on gender differences. Their findings support the notion that social factors in a child’s development play a large role in the gender differences that later emerge, as “gender differences were not found in infancy…but they emerged by the toddler/preschool period and in childhood”.[10] One possible explanation for this developmental difference comes from the child’s parents. In many Western cultures, for example, parents discuss and express a broader range of emotions with their daughters than their sons.[7] As children grow older, these patterns continue with their peers.

The second major argument in support of social influences on emotion expression involves the idea that a society’s gender roles reinforce gender differences. The social constructionist theory states that children grow up in the context of gender roles that naturally place them in role-specific situations, influencing their emotion expression in that context. Gender stereotypes in heteronormative societies enforce expectations for women to suppress anger and contempt, but express other emotions using words and facial expressions. Simultaneously, these same cultural norms discourage men from verbally expressing emotions, with the exception of anger or contempt (Wester, Pressly, & Heesacker, 2002).[7] As an adaptive feature, regulation of expression of emotion involves the consideration the social demands of any given situation. Studies have shown that “fewer gender differences in emotion expression may be found when children are with someone they trust and know well than when children are with an unfamiliar person” (Chaplin & Aldao, 2012).[10] Generally, people are trained to behave in a “socially acceptable” way around strangers or acquaintances, suggesting that the social context of an environment can shape the levels of emotion expression.

Biological factors also play a role in influencing emotion expression. One central biological argument is related to cognitive differences between genders. In a 2008 study using functional magnetic reasoning imaging (fMRI) to monitor brain activity in participants, researchers found that men and women differ in neural responses when experiencing negative emotions. “Compared with women, men showed lesser increases in prefrontal regions that are associated with reappraisal, greater decreases in the amygdala, which is associated with emotional responding, and lesser engagement of ventral striatal regions, which are associated with reward processing” (McRae, Ochsner, Mauss, Gabrieli, & Gross, 2008).[2] The way that male and female brains respond to emotions likely impacts the expression of those emotions.

The biological roots of gender differences interact with the social environment in various ways. Biological theorists propose that females and males have innate differences that exist at birth, but unfold with age and maturation in response to interactions with their specific environments (Chaplin & Aldao, 2012).[10] An important argument for this viewpoint is that “gender differences in emotion expression are the result of a combination of biologically based temperamental predispositions and the socialization of boys and girls to adopt gender-related display rules for emotion expression”.[11] It has been suggested that even infant males display higher levels of activity and arousal than do infant girls as well as a lower ability for language and behavior inhibitory controls, which are biologically-based characteristics. This “nature” argument interacts with “nurture” in that “parents and other socialization agents may respond to boys in ways that dampen emotional expressiveness…as a way to down-regulate their high emotional arousal and activity levels”.[10] On the other hand, girls are encouraged to utilize their higher vocabulary and communication skills to verbally express their emotions to parents and other adults, which would also highlight expression differences between genders.

Controversies

Emotions are complex and involve different components, such as physiological arousal, expressive behaviors, and conscious experience. While the expressive component of emotion has been widely studied, it remains unclear whether or not men and women differ in other aspects of emotion. Most researchers agree that women are more emotionally expressive, but not that they experience more emotions than men do.[1] Some studies have shown that women are more likely to produce inauthentic smiles than men do, while others have shown the opposite. This debate is significant because emotion can be generated by adopting an action that is associated with a particular emotion, such as smiling and speaking softly . [5]

A possible explanation is that both men and women’s emotional expressiveness is susceptible to social factors. Men and women may be reinforced by social and cultural standards to express emotions differently, but it is not necessarily true in terms of experiencing emotions. For instance, studies suggest that women often occupy roles that conform to feminine display rules, which require them to amplify their emotional response to impress others.[8]

Conclusion

Many societies seem to have accepted the popular viewpoint that women are simply more emotional than men are. However, various psychological and cultural studies have revealed that our reality is much more complicated than that. Close examination of recent research has led us to believe that, when it comes to emotion, our perceptions of gender differences and the behavior we observe are heavily influenced by social factors. Studies suggest that parents may encourage or discourage certain emotions in their children as early as infancy. Whether these tendencies are conscious or subconscious, many experts agree that different rules are taught to boys and girls regarding what emotions to express and how to appropriately express them. For example, women are, in fact, more likely to accurately express happiness while men are more likely to accurately express anger; studies show that that the respective genders are praised for fulfilling this expectation, while they may be met with disapproval if they fail to behave in the way deemed appropriate for their sex. Evidently, as social animals, humans are strongly pressured by these reactions. However, it is our responsibility to be conscious of these differences, their causes, and the potential shortcomings of this system. By understanding and managing our emotions, we can begin to develop healthier relationships, both with ourselves and with others.

See also

References

  1. 1 2 3 Kring, A. M., & Gordon, A. H. (1998). Sex differences in emotion: expression, experience, and physiology. Journal of personality and social psychology, 74(3), 686. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.686
  2. 1 2 McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., Mauss, I. B., Gabrieli, J. J., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Gender differences in emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 11(2), 143-162.
  3. Wester, S., Vogel, D., Pressly, P., & Heesacker, M. (2002). Sex Differences in Emotion: A Critical Review of the Literature and Implications for Counseling Psychology. The Counseling Psychologist, 30(4), 630-652.
  4. 1 2 3 Jansz, J. (2000). Masculine identity and restrictive emotionality. Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives, 166-186.
  5. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2008). Gender and emotion in context. Handbook of emotions, 3, 395-408.
  6. Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2000). The relation between gender and emotion in different cultures. In A. H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 71–98). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  7. 1 2 3 Wester, S., Vogel, D., Pressly, P., & Heesacker, M. (2002). Sex Differences in Emotion: A Critical Review of the Literature and Implications for Counseling Psychology. The Counseling Psychologist, 30(4), 630-652. Retrieved from http://www.sagepub.com/counselingstudy/Journal%20Articles/Wester.pdf
  8. 1 2 3 Derks, Daantje, Agneta H. Fischer, and Arjan E.R. Bos. The role of emotion in computer-mediated communication: A review. In Computers in Human Behavior, May 2008, Vol.24, pp.766-785. DOI: 10.1605/01.216-0003232836.2008
  9. Coats, E. J., & Feldman, R. S. (1996). Gender differences in nonverbal correlates of social status. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(10), 1014-1022.
  10. 1 2 3 4 5 Chaplin, T., & Aldao, A. (2012). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 735-765. Retrieved April 5, 2015, from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/features/bul-a0030737.pdf
  11. Brody, L. (1997). Gender and Emotion: Beyond Stereotypes. Journal of Social Issues, 53(2), 369-394. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1997.tb02448.x
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Saturday, April 09, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.