General semantics

For semantics in general, see Semantics.
Not to be confused with Generative semantics.

General semantics is a self improvement and therapy program begun in the 1920s that seeks to regulate human mental habits and behaviors. After partial launches under the names human engineering and humanology,[1] Polish-American originator Alfred Korzybski[2] (18791950) fully launched the program as general semantics in 1933 with the publication of Science and Sanity: An Introduction to Non-Aristotelian Systems and General Semantics.

The sourcebook for general semantics, Science and Sanity, presents general semantics as both a theoretical and a practical system whose adoption can reliably alter human behavior in the direction of greater sanity. Starting around 1940, university English professor S.I. Hayakawa (19061992), speech professor Wendell Johnson, speech professor Irving J. Lee, and others assembled elements of general semantics into a package suitable for incorporation into mainstream communications curricula. The Institute of General Semantics, which Korzybski and co-workers founded in 1938,[3] continues today. General Semantics as a movement has waned considerably since the 1950s, although many of its ideas live on in other movements, such as Neuro Linguistic Programming.[4][5] and Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy.[6]

Overview

"Identification" and "the silent level"

In the 1946 "Silent and Verbal Levels" diagram,[7] the arrows and boxes denote ordered stages in human neuro-evaluative processing that happens in an instant. Although newer knowledge in biology has more sharply defined what the text in these 1946 boxes labels "electro-colloidal,"[8] the diagram remains, as Korzybski wrote in his last published paper in 1950, "satisfactory for our purpose of explaining briefly the most general and important points."[9] General semantics postulates that most people "identify," or fail to differentiate the serial stages or "levels" within their own neuro-evaluative processing. "Most people," Korzybski wrote, "identify in value levels I, II, III, and IV and react as if our verbalizations about the first three levels were 'it.' Whatever we may say something 'is' obviously is not the 'something' on the silent levels."[9]

Institute of General Semantics "Silent and Verbal Levels" diagram, circa 1946[7]

By making it a 'mental' habit to find and keep one's bearings among the ordered stages, general semantics training seeks to sharpen internal orientation much as a GPS device may sharpen external orientation. Once trained, general semanticists affirm, a person will act, respond, and make decisions more appropriate to any given set of happenings. Although producing saliva constitutes an appropriate response when lemon juice drips onto the tongue, a person has inappropriately identified when an imagined lemon or the word "lemon" triggers a salivation response.

"Once we differentiate, differentiation becomes the denial of identity," Korzybski wrote in Science and Sanity. "Once we discriminate among the objective and verbal levels, we learn 'silence' on the unspeakable objective levels, and so introduce a most beneficial neurological 'delay'engage the cortex to perform its natural function."[10] British-American philosopher Max Black, an influential critic of general semantics, called this neurological delay the "central aim" of general semantics training, "so that in responding to verbal or nonverbal stimuli, we are aware of what it is that we are doing."[11]

In the 21st century, the physiology underlying identification and the neurological delay is thought to involve autoassociative memory, a neural mechanism crucial to intelligence.[12] Briefly explained, autoassociative memory retrieves previously stored representations that most closely conform to any current incoming pattern (level II in the general semantics diagram) arriving from the senses. According to the memory-prediction model for intelligence, if the stored representations resolve the arriving patterns, this constitutes "understanding," and brain activity shifts from evaluation to triggering motor responses. When the retrieved representations do not sufficiently resolve newly arrived patterns, evaluating persists, engaging higher layers of the cortex in an ongoing pursuit of resolution. The additional time required for signals to travel up and down the cortical hierarchy[13] constitutes what general semantics calls a "beneficial neurological delay."[14]

Abstracting and consciousness of abstracting

Identification prevents what general semantics seeks to promote: the additional cortical processing experienced as a delay. Korzybski called his remedy for identification "consciousness of abstracting." [15] The term "abstracting" is used ubiquitously in Science and Sanity. Korzybski's use of the term is somewhat unique and requires study to understand his meaning. He discussed the problem of identification in terms of "confusions of orders of abstractions" and "lack of consciousness of abstracting." [16] To be conscious of abstracting is to differentiate among the "levels" described above, levels II-IV being abstractions of level I (whatever level I "is"all we really get are abstractions). The techniques Korzybski prescribed to help a person develop consciousness of abstracting he called "extensional devices." [17]

Extensional devices

Satisfactory accounts of general semantics extensional devices can be found easily.[18] This article seeks to explain briefly only the "indexing" devices. Suppose you teach in a school or university. Students enter your classroom on the first day of a new term, and, if you identify these new students to a memory association retrieved by your brain, you under-engage your powers of observation and your cortex. Indexing makes explicit a differentiating of studentsthis term from studentsprior terms. You survey the new students, and indexing explicitly differentiates student1 from student2 from student3, etc. Suppose you recognize one studentcall her Annafrom a prior course in which Anna either excelled or did poorly. Again, you escape identification by your indexed awareness that Annathis term, this course is different from Annathat term, that course. Not identifying, you both expand and sharpen your apprehension of "students" with an awareness rooted in fresh silent-level observations.[19]

Language as a core concern

Autoassociative memory in the memory-prediction model describes neural operations in mammalian brains generally.[20] A special circumstance for humans arises with the introduction of language components, both as fresh stimuli and as stored representations. Language considerations figure prominently in general semantics, and three language and communications specialists who embraced general semantics, university professors and authors Hayakawa, Wendell Johnson and Neil Postman, played major roles in framing general semantics, especially for non-readers of Science and Sanity.

The science

Many recognized specialists in the knowledge areas where Korzybski claimed to have anchored general semanticsbiology, epistemology, mathematics, neurology, physics, psychiatry, etc. supported his work in his lifetime, including Cassius J. Keyser, C. B. Bridges, W. E. Ritter, P. W. Bridgman, G. E. Coghill, William Alanson White, Clarence B. Farrar, David Fairchild, and Erich Kähler. Korzybski wrote in the preface to the third edition of Science and Sanity (1947) that general semantics "turned out to be an empirical natural science."[21] But the type of existence, if any, of universals and abstract objects is an issue of serious debate within metaphysical philosophy. So Black summed up general semantics as "some hypothetical neurology fortified with dogmatic metaphysics." [22] And in 1952, two years after Korzybski died, American skeptic Martin Gardner wrote, "[Korzybski's] work moves into the realm of cultism and pseudo-science."[23]

Former Institute of General Semantics executive director Steve Stockdale has compared GS to yoga. "First, I'd say that there is little if any benefit to be gained by just knowing something about general semantics. The benefits come from maintaining an awareness of the principles and attitudes that are derived from GS and applying them as they are needed. You can sort of compare general semantics to yoga in that respect... knowing about yoga is okay, but to benefit from yoga you have to do yoga."[24] Similarly, Kenneth Burke explains Korzybski's kind of semantics contrasting it, in A Grammar of Motives, with a kind of Burkean poetry by saying "Semantics is essentially scientist, an approach to language in terms of knowledge, whereas poetic forms are kinds of action".[25][26]

History

Early attempts at validation

The First American Congress for General Semantics convened in March 1935 at the Central Washington College of Education in Ellensburg, WA. In introductory remarks to the participants, Korzybski said:

General semantics formulates a new experimental branch of natural science, underlying an empirical theory of human evaluations and orientations and involving a definite neurological mechanism, present in all humans. It discovers direct neurological methods for the stimulation of the activities of the human cerebral cortex and the direct introduction of beneficial neurological 'inhibition'....[27]

He added that general semantics "will be judged by experimentation."[28] One paper presented at the congress reported dramatic score improvements for college sophomores on standardized intelligence tests after six weeks of training by methods prescribed in Chapter 29 of Science and Sanity.[29]

Interpretation as semantics

General semantics accumulated only a few early experimental validations. In 1938, economist and writer Stuart Chase praised and popularized Korzybski in The Tyranny of Words. Chase called Korzybski "a pioneer" and described Science and Sanity as "formulating a genuine science of communication. The term which is coming into use to cover such studies is 'semantics,' matters having to do with signification or meaning."[30] Because Korzybski, in Science and Sanity, had articulated his program using "semantic" as a standalone qualifier on hundreds of pages in constructions like "semantic factors," "semantic disturbances," and especially "semantic reactions," to label the general semantics program "semantics" amounted to only a convenient shorthand.[31]

Hayakawa read The Tyranny of Words, then Science and Sanity, and in 1939 he attended a Korzybski-led workshop conducted at the newly organized Institute of General Semantics in Chicago. In the introduction to his own Language in Action, a 1941 Book of the Month Club selection, Hayakawa wrote, "[Korzybski's] principles have in one way or another influenced almost every page of this book...."[32] But, Hayakawa followed Chase's lead in interpreting general semantics as making communication its defining concern. When Hayakawa co-founded the Society for General Semantics and its publication ETC.:A Review of General Semantics in 1943he would continue to edit ETC. until 1970Korzybski and his followers at the Institute of General Semantics began to complain that Hayakawa had wrongly coopted general semantics.[33] In 1985, Hayakawa gave this defense to an interviewer: "I wanted to treat general semantics as a subject, in the same sense that there's a scientific concept known as gravitation, which is independent of Isaac Newton. So after a while, you don't talk about Newton anymore; you talk about gravitation. You talk about semantics and not Korzybskian semantics." [34]

Lowered sights

The regimen in the Institute's seminars, greatly expanded as team-taught seminar-workshops starting in 1944, continued to develop following the prescriptions laid down in Chapter XXIX of Science and Sanity. The structural differential, patented by Korzybski in the 1920s, remained among the chief training aids to help students reach "the silent level," a prerequisite for achieving "neurological delay." Innovations in the seminar-workshops included a new "neuro-relaxation" component, led by dancer and Institute editorial secretary Charlotte Schuchardt (19092002).

But although many people were introduced to general semanticsperhaps the majority through Hayakawa's more limited 'semantics'superficial lip service seemed more common than the deep internalization that Korzybski and his co-workers at the Institute aimed for. Marjorie Kendig (18921981), probably Korzybski's closest co-worker, director of the Institute after his death, and editor of his posthumously published Collected Writings: 1920-1950, wrote in 1968:

I would guess that I have known about 30 individuals who have in some degree adequately, by my standards, mastered this highly general, very simple, very difficult system of orientation and method of evaluatingreversing as it must all our cultural conditioning, neurological canalization, etc.... To me the great error Korzybski madeand I carried on, financial necessityand for which we pay the price today in many criticisms, consisted in not restricting ourselves to training very thoroughly a very few people who would be competent to utilize the discipline in various fields and to train others. We should have done this before encouraging anyone to popularize or spread the word (horrid phrase) in societies for general semantics, by talking about general semantics instead of learning, using, etc. the methodology to change our essential epistemological assumptions, premises, etc. (unconscious or conscious), i.e. the un-learning basic to learning to learn.
Yes, large numbers of people do enjoy making a philosophy of general semantics. This saves them the pain of rigorous training so simple and general and limited that it seems obvious when said, yet so difficult.[35]

Successors at the Institute of General Semantics continued for many years along the founders' path. Stuart Mayper (19161997), who studied under Karl Popper, introduced Popper's principle of falsifiability into the seminar-workshops he led at the Institute starting in 1977. More modest pronouncements gradually replaced Korzybski's claims that general semantics can change human nature and introduce an era of universal human agreement. In 2000, Robert Pula (19282004), whose roles at the Institute over three decades included Institute director, editor-in-chief of the Institute's General Semantics Bulletin, and leader of the seminar-workshops, characterized Korzybski's legacy as a "contribution toward the improvement of human evaluating, to the amelioration of human woe...."[36]

Hayakawa died in 1992. The Society for General Semantics merged into the Institute of General Semantics in 2003. In 2007, Martin Levinson, president of the Institute's Board of Trustees, teamed with Paul D. Johnston, executive director of the Society at the date of the merger, to teach general semantics with a light-hearted Practical Fairy Tales for Everyday Living.[37] The Institute currently offers no training workshops.

Other institutions supporting or promoting general semantics in the 21st century include the New York Society for General Semantics,[38] the European Society for General Semantics,[39] the Australian General Semantics Society,[40] and the Balvant Parekh Centre for General Semantics and Other Human Sciences (Baroda, India).[41]

The major premises

Connections to other disciplines

General semantics has important links with analytic philosophy and the philosophy of science; it could be characterized without too much distortion as applied analytic philosophy. The influence of Ludwig Wittgenstein and the Vienna Circle, and of early operationalists and pragmatists such as Charles Sanders Peirce, is particularly clear in the foundational ideas of general semantics. Korzybski himself acknowledged many of these influences.

The concept of "silence on the objective level" attributed to Korzybski and his insistence on consciousness of abstracting are parallel to some central ideas in Zen Buddhism. Korzybski is not recorded to have acknowledged any influence from this quarter, but he formulated general semantics during the same years that the first popularizations of Zen were becoming part of the intellectual currency of educated speakers of English. On the other hand, later Zen-popularizer Alan Watts was influenced by ideas from general semantics.

L. Ron Hubbard is widely believed to have used the theory in his creation of Dianetics and later to have incorporated it into Scientology, and acknowledges this in several texts; the first of these two movements in turn introduced general semantics to a wider audience in the early 1950s, including popular science fiction writer A. E. van Vogt, personal growth theorist Harvey Jackins and his movement Re-evaluation Counseling and movements like Gestalt therapy. The founders of these movements did not themselves credit Korzybski for their ideas.

General semantics has survived most profoundly in the cognitive therapies that emerged in the 1950s and 1960s. Albert Ellis (1913–2007), who developed Rational emotive behavior therapy, acknowledged influence from general semantics and delivered the Alfred Korzybski Memorial Lecture in 1991. The Bruges (Belgium) center for Solution Focused Therapy operates under the name Korzybski Institute Training and Research Center.[43] George Kelly, founder of Personal Construct Psychology, was influenced by general semantics.[44] Frederick Perls and Paul Goodman, founders of Gestalt therapy are said to have been influenced by Korzybski [45] Wendell Johnson wrote "People in Quandaries: The Semantics of Personal Adjustment" in 1946, which stands as the first attempt to form a therapy from general semantics

Ray Solomonoff (July 25, 1926 – December 7, 2009) was influenced [46] by Korzybski. Solomonoff was the inventor of algorithmic probability, and founder of algorithmic information theory (aka Kolmogorov complexity). Another scientist influenced by Korzybski (verbal testimony) is Paul Vitanyi (born July 21, 1944), a scientist in the theory of computation.

During the 1940s, 1950s, and 1960s, general semantics entered the idiom of science fiction, most notably through the works of A. E. van Vogt, The World of Null-A and its sequels, Robert A. Heinlein, Gulf, and Frank Herbert, in Dune [47] and Whipping Star.[48] The ideas of general semantics became a sufficiently important part of the shared intellectual toolkit of genre science fiction to merit parody by Damon Knight and others; they have since shown a tendency to reappear in the work of more recent writers such as Samuel R. Delany, Suzette Haden Elgin and Robert Anton Wilson. In 2008, John Wright extended van Vogt's Null-A series with Null-A Continuum. William Burroughs references Korzybski's time binding principle in his essay The Electronic Revolution, and elsewhere.

Neil Postman, founder of New York University's media ecology program in 1971, edited ETC.: A Review of General Semantics from 1976 to 1986. Postman's student Lance Strate, a co-founder of the Media Ecology Association,[49] served as executive director of the Institute of General Semantics from 2007 to 2010.

See also

Related books

Notes

  1. Korzybski, Alfred (1974). Time-Binding: The General Theory. Two Papers 19241926. Lakeville, CT: Institute of General Semantics. pp. (5), 54.
  2. Kodish, Bruce I. (2011). Korzybski: A Biography. Pasadena, CA: Extensional Publishing. p. 257. ISBN 978-0-9700664-0-4.
  3. Kodish, Bruce I. Korzybski: A Biography, p. 440.
  4. Linder-Pelz, S. and Hall, L.M., 2007. The theoretical roots of NLP-based coaching. The Coaching Psychologist, 3(1), pp.12-17.
  5. Witkowski, Tomasz. "A review of research findings on neuro-linguistic programming." Scientific Review of Mental Health Practice (2011)
  6. Ellis, Albert. "General Semantics and Rational-Emotive Therapy." General Semantics Bulletin, 1993, Number 58. Institute of General Semantics, Englewood, NJ. pp. 12-28.
  7. 1 2 Kendig, M., "Alfred Korzybski's 'An Extensional Analysis of the Process of Abstracting from an Electro-Colloidal Non-Aristotelian Point of View.'" General Semantics Bulletin, AutumnWinter 195051, Numbers Four & Five. Institute of General Semantics, Lakeville, CT. pp. 910.
  8. Wright, Barbara E., "The Hereditary-Environment Continuum: Holistic Approaches at 'One Point in Time' and in 'All Time'". General Semantics Bulletin, 1986, Number 52. Institute of General Semantics, Englewood, NJ. pp. 4344. Wright, professor of biology at the University of Montana, wrote, "In the 1930s, when Korzybski wrote about colloids, they represented the frontier of our emerging knowledge about the complex interdependence of cellular structures and biochemical systems.... Today, the word colloid is used very rarely; I could not find it in the indices of several current textbooks of biochemistry. Perhaps this change in usage came about because we now know so much more about individual kinds of colloids; the word became so all-inclusive as to lose its usefulness."
  9. 1 2 Blake, Robert R. and Glenn V. Ramsey, editors (1951). Perception: An Approach to Personality. New York: Ronald Press, pp. 170–205; chapter 7: "The Role of Language in the Perceptual Process" by Alfred Korzybski, p. 172.
  10. Korzybski, Science and Sanity (5th ed.), p. 404.
  11. Black, Max. Language and Philosophy: Studies in Method. Ithaca, NY.: Cornell University Press. p. 239. Black's chapter about general semantics originated as an April 1946 lecture at the State University of Iowa.
  12. Hawkins, Jeff (2004). On Intelligence. New York: Henry Holt. pp. 2931 and 7375. ISBN 978-0-8050-7456-7.
  13. Hawkins, Jeff. On Intelligence. pp. 166167
  14. For a short summary of the brain activity postulated in the memory-prediction model, see Coert Visser (2004). "Understanding Intelligence". Retrieved 2011-10-03.
  15. Korzybski, Alfred. Science and Sanity (5th ed.). p. 500
  16. Korzybski, Alfred. Science and Sanity (5th ed.). p. 36
  17. Korzybski, Alfred. Science and Sanity (5th ed.). p. lx
  18. For example, a source reference for "scare quotes" and other extensional devices not treated in this article is Postman, Neil. "Alfred Korzybski," ETC: A Review of General Semantics, Winter 2003
  19. Encyclopædia Britannica (1947). 10 Eventful Years: 1937 through 1946. Chicago: Encyclopædia Britannica. Volume 4, pp. 2932. "Semantics: General Semantics." The article, written by S.I. Hayakawa, states, "Korzybski did not intend these extensional devices simply as things to say by rote or to sprinkle through one's writing. Each of them was intended to point beyond itself to subverbal levelsto observing and feeling and absorbing as directly perceived data the nonlinguistic actualities...." Explaining the name selection for the devices, Hayakawa wrote, "Appropriating from formal logic the term 'extension,' which means the aggregate of things denoted by a term (as opposed to 'intension,' the qualities of properties implied by the term), he [Korzybski] called his rules extensional devices."
  20. Hawkins, Jeff. On Intelligence. p. 99
  21. Korzybski, Alfred. Science and Sanity (5th ed.). p. xxxiv
  22. Black, Max. Language and Philosophy: Studies in Method. p. 246.
  23. Gardner, Martin (1957). Fads and Fallacies in the Name of Science. New York: Dover Publications. ch. 23, pp. 280-291.
  24. "FOLLY with Steve Stockdale". FollyMag. June 2007. Retrieved 2011-10-03. Stockdale: "First, I'd say that there is little if any benefit to be gained by just 'knowing' something about general semantics. The benefits come from maintaining an awareness of the principles and attitudes that are derived from GS and applying them as they are needed. You can sort of compare general semantics to yoga in that respect... knowing about yoga is okay, but to benefit from yoga you have to 'do' yoga." Reprinted in Stockdale, Steve (2009). Here's Something about General Semantics: A Primer for Making Sense of Your World. Santa Fe, NM: Steve Stockdale. p. 36. ISBN 978-0-9824645-0-2
  25. Burke, Kenneth (1945). "Scholarly outline of Burke's "A Grammar of Motives"". University of California Press. [Burke] would encourage the "delayed response" (p.238). Korzybski’s technique recommends that an individual interpose a "moment of delay" between the "Stimulus and the Response" in order to control meaning (p.239). According to Burke, Korzybski’s doctrine of the delayed action, as based on the ‘consciousness of abstracting,’ involves the fact that any term for an object puts the object in a class of similar objects" (p.240). Burke points out that Korzybski’s technique falls short with regard to the "analysis of poetic forms": "For ‘semantics’ is essentially scientist, an approach to language in terms of knowledge, whereas poetic forms are kinds of action" (p.240).
  26. Burke, Kenneth (1945). A Grammar of Motives. University of California Press. pp. 238–242.
  27. Korzybski, Alfred. "An Outline of General Semantics." In Papers from the First American Congress for General Semantics, collected and arranged by Hansell Baugh (1938). New York: Arrow Editions. p. 1.
  28. Korzybski, Alfred. "An Outline of General Semantics." In Papers from the First American Congress for General Semantics. p. 4.
  29. Trainor, Joseph C. "Experimental Results of Training in General Semantics upon Intelligence Test Scores." In Papers from the First American Congress for General Semantics, pp. 5862.
  30. Chase, Stuart (1966). The Tyranny of Words. New York: Harcourt, Brace. p. 7.
  31. Kodish, Bruce I. Korzybski: A Biography. pp. 343, 439.
  32. Hayakawa, S. I. (1941). Language in Action. New York: Harcourt, Brace. p. viii.
  33. Kodish, Bruce I. Korzybski: A Biography, p. 554.
  34. Shearer, Julie Gordon (1989). "From Semantics to the U.S. Senate: S. I. Hayakawa." This interview has been posted through the Online Archive of California. The cited statement by Hayakawa can be located via an internet search for Shearer + Hayakawa + "Keeping ETC. Independent of Korzybski" .
  35. Kendig, Marjorie. "Reflections on the State of the Discipline, 1968". General Semantics Bulletin, 1983, Number 50. Institute of General Semantics, Baltimore, MD. p. 68.
  36. Pula, Robert P. (2000). A General-Semantics Glossary: Pula's Guide for the Perplexed. Concord, CA: International Society for General Semantics. p. viii. ISBN 0-918970-49-0.
  37. Levinson, Martin H., Illustrations by Paul D. Johnston (2007). Practical Fairy Tales for Everyday Living. Lincoln, NE: iUniverse. ISBN 978-0-595-42140-4.
  38. http://www.nysgs.org
  39. http://esgs.free.fr/uk
  40. "The Australian General Semantics Society".
  41. http://balvantparekhcentre.org.in
  42. Gorman, Margaret (1962). General Semantics and Contemporary Thomism. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln. p. 31.
  43. "Korzybski International".
  44. http://www.pcp-net.org/encyclopaedia/kelly.html
  45. "Alfred Korzybski and Gestalt Therapy".
  46. "The Discovery of Algorithmic Probability," Journal of Computer and System Sciences, Vol 55, No. 1, pp. 73-88 (pdf version)
  47. Tim O'Reilly. Frank Herbert. New York, NY: Frederick Ungar Publishing Co., Inc. ,1981. (pp.59-60) ISBN 0-8044-2666-X . "Herbert had studied general semantics in San Francisco at about the time he was writing Dune. (At one point, he worked as a ghostwriter for a nationally syndicated column by S. I. Hayakawa, one of the foremost proponents of general semantics.)"
  48. O'Reilly,1981, (p.180) "The influence of General Semantics is particularly obvious in Whipping Star"...
  49. "Media Ecology Association".

Other sources not cited in the article

Further reading

Related academic articles

External links

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Saturday, March 12, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.