Hering illusion
The Hering illusion is one of the geometrical-optical illusions and was discovered by the German physiologist Ewald Hering in 1861.[1] When two straight and parallel lines are presented in front of radial background (like the spokes of a bicycle), the lines appear as if they were bowed outwards. The Orbison illusion is one of its variants, while the Wundt illusion produces a similar, but inverted effect.
There are several possible explanations for why perceptual distortion produced by the radiating pattern. The illusion was ascribed by Hering to an overestimation of the angle made at the points of intersection. If true, it is interesting that what yields is the straightness of the parallel lines and not of the radiating lines, implying that there is a hierarchical ordering among components of such illusion.[2] Others have suggested that angle overestimation results from lateral inhibition in visual cortex,[3] while others have postulated a bias inherent in extrapolating 3D angle information from 2D projections.[4][5]
A different framework suggests that the Hering illusion (and several other geometric illusions) are caused by temporal delays with which the visual system must cope.[6][7] In this framework, the visual system extrapolates current information to “perceive the present”: instead of providing a conscious image of how the world was ~100 ms in the past (when signals first struck the retina), the visual system estimates how the world is likely to look in the next moment. In the case of the Hering illusion, the radial lines trick the visual system into thinking it is moving forward. Since we are not actually moving and the figure is static, we misperceive the straight lines as curved—as they would appear in the next moment.
It is possible that both frameworks are compatible. The Hering illusion can also be induced by a background of optic flow (such as dots flowing out from the center of a screen, creating the illusion of forward motion through a starfield). Importantly, the bowing direction is the same whether the flow moves inward or outward.[8] This result is consistent with a role for networks of visual orientation-tuned neurons (e.g., simple cells in primary visual cortex) in the spatial warping. In this framework, under the common condition of forward ego-motion, it is possible that spatial warping counteracts the disadvantage of neural latencies. However, it also demonstrates that any spatial warping that counteracts neural delays is not a precise, on-the-fly computation, but instead a heuristic achieved by a simple mechanism that succeeds under normal circumstances.
References
Wikimedia Commons has media related to Hering illusion. |
- ↑ Hering, E. (1861). Beiträge zur Physiologie. I. Zur Lehre vom Ortssinne der Netzhaut. Leipzig: Engelmann
- ↑ Westheimer G (2008). "Geometrical-optical illusions and the neural representation of space". Vision Res 48 (20): 2128–2142. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.05.016. PMID 18606433.
- ↑ Blakemore et al., 1970
- ↑ Nundy et al., 2000
- ↑ Howe CQ, Purves D; Purves (2005). "Natural-scene geometry predicts the perception of angles and line orientation". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102 (4): 1228–1233. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.1228H. doi:10.1073/pnas.0409311102. PMC 544621. PMID 15657143.
- ↑ Changizi, 2001
- ↑ Changizi and Widders, 2002
- ↑ Vaughn DA, Eagleman DM (2013). "Spatial warping by oriented line detectors can counteract neural delays". Frontiers in Psychology. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00794.
|