Jargon aphasia

Jargon aphasia is a type of fluent aphasia in which an individual's speech is incomprehensible, but appears to make sense to the individual. Persons experiencing this condition will either replace a desired word with another that sounds or looks like the original one, or has some other connection to it, or they will replace it with random sounds. Accordingly, persons with jargon aphasia often use neologisms, and may perseverate if they try to replace the words they can't find with sounds.

The Discovery of Jargon

Hughlings Jackson is believed to have been the person who initially contributed the term “jargon” to aphasiology. He used this term not to distinguish a separate type of aphasia, but to describe the language output of certain people that was meaningless and incomprehensible to the listener, although it appeared to have some meaning for the speaker[3].

Causes and Development of Aphasia

People affected by jargon aphasia usually are elderly and/or people who have damage to the neural pathways of certain parts of the brain. This is usually the result of the following conditions[2]:

Since jargon is associated with fluent (Wernicke’s) aphasia, it is usually caused by damage of the temporal lobe, and more specifically, Wernicke’s area. After the condition is diagnosed, a Computerized Tomography (CT) or Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) scan is typically used to determine the location and severity of the brain damage that has caused the aphasia[2].

There have been cases in which aphasia has developed after damage to only the right hemisphere of the brain. These cases are few and far between, and usually involve unique circumstances for the individual. Most commonly, these results can stem from brain organization that is different than the general population, or a heavier than normal reliance on the right hemisphere of the brain[7].

Symptoms and Diagnosis

Someone with jargon aphasia may exhibit the following behaviors[4]:

Some of the specific types of language errors that occur are[5]:

Lexical (real word):

Semantic - Real word that was semantically related to target. ("dog" instead of "cat")

Formal - Real word that shared either the initial phoneme or at least 50% or more phonemes with target. ("dog" instead of "desk")

Mixed - Real word that was both semantically and phonologically related to target. ("bicycle" instead of "motorcycle")

Visual - Real word of an item similar in visual form to the target. ("ball" instead of "orange")

Unrelated - Real word that was not related to the target in any obvious way. ("dog" instead of "apple")

Non-lexical (nonword):

Phonological - Nonword that shared either the initial phoneme or at least 50% of phonemes with target. ("deg" instead of "dog")

Neologistic - Nonword not reaching the criterion for phonological relatedness (i.e., sharing less than 50% of phonemes with the target and with a different initial phoneme). Nonwords that are pseudo compound words. ("kib" instead of "dog")

Other Errors:

Don’t know - Indication that response was unknown or if item was not responded to at all. ("I don’t know" or silence)

Description - Attempts to describe as opposed to name item. (Multiple word responses)

Jargon aphasia must be diagnosed through a series of tests. Since the number of individuals that have aphasia after suffering a stroke is high, a test is usually carried out soon after the stroke occurs. There is a list of basic exercises to help assess a person’s language skills, such as:

There is also a common test used, called the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination test, which incorporates exercises that extensively review the person’s language skills[2].

Treatments

The only way to treat aphasia is with speech and language therapy (SLT). It will not completely restore the person’s prior level of communication, but SLT can lead to a massive improvement of jargon aphasia. Recipients of this treatment typically achieve better use of residual language abilities, improved language skills, and the ability to communicate in a different way by making up for missing words in their speech[2].

One specific method that has shown to lead to improvements with certain symptoms is “phonological component analysis”, or PCA for short. Participants in PCA therapy tend to improve in the ability to name specific items that they are test on, as well as the decrease in use of nonwords to describe said items. Seeing promising results from this type of therapy has led to much optimism in hopes of developing more treatment methods for jargon aphasia[6].

Other Meanings and Types

There are lots of different meanings when people refer to jargon aphasia. Since Hughling Jackson’s time, it has covered a broad range of similar verbal behaviors and has been used to describe a multitude of different aphiastic disturbances. Some of the behaviors are described as the person having speaking in a “confused, unintelligible language”, “a strange, outlandish, or barbarous dialect”, “a hybrid language”, and can be referred to as a pretentious language marked by circumlocutions and long words. Observation of these behaviors has led to a branching of different types of jargon.

Neologistic Jargon is the production of language containing non-existent words that are not related to what the person is attempting to convey.

Phonemic Jargon is the production of language containing inappropriate words that are phonemically related to what the person is attempting to convey.

Semanic Jargon is the production of language which is devoid of content and consists of real words that are inappropriate given the context of the situation. All of these types of jargon are seen in fluent aphasia, which can more commonly be addressed as Wernicke’s aphasia[1,3].

Contradictions and Different Viewpoints

Weinstein: Weinstein’s viewpoint of jargon illustrates just the basic rambling and incoherent but structurally intact speech. It does not include details about neologisms and paraphasias. He and his coworkers view jargon as a positive symptom of aphasia, and as an adaptive behavior and form of denial, or anosognosia, in the presence of language deficit.

Schuell: Shuell views jargon as the result of severe impairment in the recall of learned auditory patterns and imperfect auditory feedback processes. The lack of control of verbal output is related to reduced auditory input.

Cohn and Neumann: Their viewpoint is that jargon results from the disruption of the sequential ordering of speech.

Alajouanine: Alajouanine came up with the most unusual viewpoint of all. He pointed out that the reason for jargon cannot be found in the breakdown of the intrinsic speech structure itself. He stressed that incomprehensibility and lack of meaning, rather than articulatory abnormality or lack of proper grammatical sequencing were the essence of jargon. He often spoke of a “suppression of the semantic values of language” in jargon[3].

References

    [1] [2] [3] [4] [5] [6] [7]

    Further reading

    case histories


    1. 1.Rohrer JD, Rossor MN, Warren JD. Neologistic jargon aphasia and agraphia in primary progressive aphasia. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2009;277(1-2):155-159. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2008.10.014.
    2. 2.Paddock M. What is aphasia? What causes aphasia?. Medical News Today. 2014. Available at: http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/217487.php#post. Accessed May 1, 2015.
    3. 3.Rau, Marie T. Jargon Aphasia: A confusing state of affairs. Veterans Administration Hospital.
    4. 4.Marshall, Jane. Jargon aphasia: What have we learned? Psychology Press. May 1, 2006. Volume 20, issue 5.
    5. 5.Moses, Melanie S; Nickels, Lyndsey A; Sheard, Christine. Disentangling the Web: Neologistic Perseverative Errors in Jargon Aphasia. Psychology Press. December 1, 2004. Volume 10, issue 6.
    6. 6.Bose, Arpita. Phonological therapy in jargon aphasia: effects on naming and neologisms. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. September 21, 2013. Volume 48, issue 5.
    7. 7.Coppens, P; Hungerford, S; Yamaguchi, S; Yamadori, A (December 2002). "Crossed aphasia: an analysis of the symptoms, their frequency, and a comparison with left-hemisphere aphasia symptomatology.". Brain and Language 83 (3): 425–63. doi:10.1016/s0093-934x(02)00510-2. PMID 12468397.
    This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Monday, March 14, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.