Language acquisition by deaf children
In the United States, one in a thousand children is born profoundly deaf. Despite their inability to hear, communication and language acquisition are fundamental to their general cognitive development and their engagement with their surroundings.The language that deaf community use in America is called American Sign Language(ASL). In the same way as hearing children do, deaf children, although deprived of language, acquire linguistic skills to communicate. In 1957, Noam Chomsky, the pioneer of the nativist theory of language acquisition, claimed that all humans are born with an innate capacity for language, in other words, a language acquisition device. The fact that deaf children are able to communicate through sign language supports his view for an innate capability to communicate.
Role of the environment
Deaf-children born to deaf parents
Some deaf children’s lag in language development and subsequent struggles in school are not solely attributable to deafness. Deaf children born to deaf parents acquire sign language just as quickly and with as much effort as hearing children acquire spoken language. Although they may communicate less frequently than their hearing counterparts, deaf mothers’ language is made more accessible and thereby more salient to their children. Furthermore, deaf children born to deaf parents surpass those born to hearing parents in regards to academic performance.
Deaf children born to hearing parents
More than 90% of deaf children are born to hearing parents. Due to an entirely different view and experience of the world, this often leads to difficulties in parent-child communication. Signals from the environment help children create a language system. A child must be inserted into the language-learning situation equipped with certain propensities in order to be able to take advantage of ‘hints’ offered by the environment. Such environmental cues include other people pointing at things, nodding or shaking their heads, or performing certain actions that show intentions and meaning.
Language acquisition
Making language accessible
Early intervention programs are based on research of non-deaf children’s acquisition of spoken language. Therefore, some strategies are used that, while appropriate for hearing children, are utterly futile when used with deaf children. Some of these strategies include attempting to direct attention by speaking the child’s name. For parents with deaf children, joint attention (an important component to language development) is problematic. Hearing children can watch their environment and listen to an adult comment on it. However, deaf children have to switch their attention back and forth between stimuli. Since observation and language occur sequentially rather than simultaneously for deaf children, the association is less obvious and the necessary cognitive processing to make these connections are more difficult. To lessen these demands, a parent can use certain strategies to make language more accessible to their deaf children. Strategies for nonverbal communication include using facial expressions and body language to show emotion and reinforce the child’s attention to their caregiver. To attract and direct a deaf child’s attention, caregivers can break his line of gaze using hand and body movements, touch, and pointing to allow language input. In order to make language salient, parents should use short, simple sentences so that the child’s attention doesn’t have to be divided for too long. Finally, to reduce the need for divided attention, a caregiver can position themselves and objects within the child’s visual field so that language and the object can be seen at the same time.
ASL (American sign language)
ASL is a human language with equal linguistic complexity and expressiveness than that of any other spoken language. It employs signs made by moving one’s hands along with one’s facial expressions and body language. Most medical professionals emphasize that deaf children only have two options: the oral route (access to spoken language only) and the manual route (sign language). They ignore that there is yet another way to communicate, that is, using sign language while simultaneously promoting English speech development. Some studies indicate that if a deaf child learns sign language, he or she will be less likely to learn spoken languages because they will lose motivation. However, Humphries insists that there is no evidence for this. Learning ASL prevents linguistic deprivation along with the social ramifications such as feelings of exclusion from the hearing community. Learning ASL at an early stage of development significantly improves a deaf child’s communication skills.
MCE (manually coded english)
MCE is a collection of sign systems, which represents the English syntactic structure in a manual way. Often, deaf people sign MCE and speak simultaneously. In this way, deaf children can learn the structure of the English language not only through the sound and lip-reading patterns of spoken English, but also through manual patterns of signed English. Although MCE is rather complicated and rigorous by nature of representing the entire English grammar manually, it helps strengthen communication between English speakers and deaf people. It is also much easier for hearing people to learn MCE rather than ASL since it is mapped on the same grammatical structure.
Deaf children and reading
According to Goldin-Meadow, reading requires two essential abilities: familiarity with a language and understanding the mapping between that language and the written word. At birth, deaf children are deficient in both. However, reading is possible if deaf children learn ASL, a linguistic code that although, not based on sounds, is still nonetheless a language. Once they have acquired ASL, deaf children learn how to map between sign language and print so that they can learn English. Several techniques are used to help bridge the gap between ASL and spoken language or the “translation process” such as sandwiching and chaining. Sandwiching consists of alternating between saying the word and signing it. Chaining consists of finger spelling a word, pointing to the spoken language version of the word and using pictorial support. Although chaining is not widely used, it creates an understanding between the visual spelling of a word and the sign language spelling of the word. This helps the child become bilingual in both ASL and spoken language. More importantly, the deaf child’s social context is crucial for nurturing his or her capacity to read. Research shows that deaf children born to deaf parents are usually better readers than deaf children born to hearing parents. This is due to the fact that deaf parents provide a strong social and emotional network and may immediately have access to the necessary resources for their child. Deaf parents already anticipate the needs of their child, having been through the same experience, as opposed to a hearing parent. Although MCE is helpful, ASL is the key component for deaf children’s reading. Deaf children who made progress in ASL and MCE also made progress in reading English, but children who only made some progress in MCE didn’t make progress in reading English. More evidence for the necessity of acquiring ASL is reflected in studies comparing reading scores and ratings of sign language performance. According to Meadow, there is a positive relationship between speech reading scores and ratings for speech, expressive finger spelling and sign usage. Contrary to popular belief, this strongly suggests that deaf children with ASL in fact do acquire strong reading skills. However, children who do not have language such as ASL to map the printed code can never read. Hence, in order for a deaf child to learn to read, he or she must know ASL beforehand. The sooner the child learns ASL the better he or she will be at developing effective communicative skills. Mastery of written language is especially important today as we are moving into the Information Age. In order to succeed, one must possess strong reading and writing skills.
Cochlear implants
A cochlear implant is placed surgically inside the cochlea, which is the part of the inner ear that converts sound to neural signals. The implant receives signals from an external device worn behind the ear and stimulates electrodes in the cochlea. These electrodes stimulate the auditory nerve directly, circumventing the hair cells that are involved in the beginning stages of auditory neural processing. Although some researchers claim that cochlear implants only helps hearing for adults or children who become deaf after having acquired language, and not as much children born deaf, recent research has shown that cochlear implants can actually promote the development of speech perception and production in prelingually deaf children. Children born deaf who received cochlear implants showed increased gains in expressive language and speech perception (determined by a variety of phoneme recognition tests) than deaf children who had not received the implant. Strong evidence indicates that the earlier the cochlear implantation is done, the less delays there are in language development for pre-linguistically deaf children. If implanted early enough deaf children can attain clear, normal spoken language.
Ethics and language acquisition
Cochlear implants have been the subject of a heated debate between those that believe deaf children should receive the implants and those that do not. Members of “Deaf World” believe this is an important ethical problem. They strongly advocate that sign language is their first or native language just as any other spoken language is for a hearing person. They do not see deafness as a deficiency in any way, but rather a normal human trait amongst a variety of different ones. Members are particularly concerned with giving deaf children the ability to hear and acquire language; they are worried that it will lead to the genocide of Deaf culture. One issue on the ethical perspective of implantation is the numerous amount of possible side effects that may present themselves after surgery. There are various severe side effects that may result from the surgery, even in some cases lessening listening capabilities, losing residual hearing or hearing sounds differently. While the surgery presents one positive solution, these side effects are not often taken into account but are significant and need to be afforded more attention.
Concluding thought
Despite criticism and controversy regarding cochlear implants, evidence demonstrates that the simultaneous implementation of implants and ASL ultimately increase a deaf child’s linguistic and communicative capabilities. As a result, deaf children have stronger chances of integrating with both deaf and hearing communicating; thereby allowing them to engage socially. For the most effective language acquisition, deaf children must be immersed in a nurturing environment that includes educational and parental support.
References
- Meier, R. (1991). Language Acquisition by Deaf Children. American Scientist (Vol. 79, pp. 60–70). Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society.
- Pinker, S.; Jackendoff, R. (2005). "The faculty of language: what's special about it?". Cognition 95: 201–236. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.004. PMID 15694646.
- Paul, P. (2009). Language and Deafness. 4th Edition. Sudbury: Jones & Bartlett Learning.
- Mohay, H., Milton L., Hindmarsh, G. & Ganley K. (1998). Deaf Mothers as Communication Models for Hearing Families with Deaf Children. In Weisel, A. (Ed.), Issues Unresolved: New Perspectives on Language and Deaf Education (pp. 76–87). Washington: Gallaudet University Press.
- Bowe, F (1998). "Language Development in Deaf Children". Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 3 (1): 73–77. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.deafed.a014342.
- Scott, S & Simms, L. (2012). Everything You Always Wanted to Know About ASL/English Bimodal Bilingual Education [Handout].
- Goldin-Meadow, S.; Mayberry, R. (2001). "How Do Profoundly Deaf Children Learn to Read?". Learning Diabilities Research & Practice 16 (4): 222–229. doi:10.1111/0938-8982.00022.
- Meadow, K (2005). "Early Manual Communication in Relation to the Deaf Child's Intellectual, Social, and Communicative Functioning". Journal of Deaf Studies in Deaf Education 10 (4): 321–329. doi:10.1093/deafed/eni035.
- Svirsky, M.; et al. (2000). "Language Development in Profoundly Deaf Children With Cochlear Implants". Psychological Science 11 (2): 153–158. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00231.
- Goldin-Meadow, S. et al. (1984). Gestural Communication in Deaf Children: The Effects and Noneffects of Parental Input on Early Language Development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (Vol. 49, pp. 1–151). Wiley.
- Examining Chomsky’s Inborn Universal Grammar Theory. (n.d.). In Boundless. Retrieved November 11, 2013.
- Cochlear Implants - Benefits and Risks of Cochlear Implants. (September 8, 2010). In U.S. Food and Drug Administration. Retrieved November 11, 2013.
- American Sign Language. (June 2011). In National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD). Retrieved November 11, 2013.
- Humphries, T. et al. (2012). Language acquisition for deaf children: Reducing the harms of zero tolerance to the use of alternative approaches. Harm Reduction Journal, 9 (16).
- Courtin, C. (2000). The Impact of Sign Language on the Cognitive Development of Deaf Children: The Case of Theories of Mind. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(3), 266-276.