Muscle memory

For the term "muscle memory" as related to strength training, see Muscle memory (strength training).

Muscle memory has been used synonymously with motor learning, which is a form of procedural memory that involves consolidating a specific motor task into memory through repetition. When a movement is repeated over time, a long-term muscle memory is created for that task, eventually allowing it to be performed without conscious effort. This process decreases the need for attention and creates maximum efficiency within the motor and memory systems. Examples of muscle memory are found in many everyday activities that become automatic and improve with practice, such as riding a bicycle, typing on a keyboard, typing in a PIN, playing a musical instrument, or martial arts.[1]

History

The origins of research for the acquisition of motor skills stem from philosophers such as Plato, Aristotle and Galen. Friedrich Bessel is a philosopher who is especially noteworthy, as he was among the first to empirically observe motor learning. Bessel tried to observe the difference in his colleagues with the method in which they recorded the transit time of stars.[2] After the break from tradition of the pre-1900s view of introspection, psychologists emphasized research and more scientific methods in observing behaviours.[3] Thereafter, numerous studies exploring the role of motor learning were conducted. Such studies included the research of handwriting, and various practice methods to maximize motor learning.[2]

Retention

The retention of motor skills, now referred to as muscle memory, also began to be of great interest in the early 1900s. Most motor skills are thought to be acquired through practice; however, mere observation of the skill has led to learning as well.[4] Research suggests we do not start off with a blank slate with regard to motor memory although we do learn most of our motor memory repertoire during our lifetime.[5] Movements such as facial expressions, which are thought to be learned, can actually be observed in children who are blind; thus there is some evidence for motor memory being genetically pre-wired.[5]

In the early stages of empirical research of motor memory Edward Thorndike, a leading pioneer in the study of motor memory, was among the first to acknowledge learning can occur without conscious awareness.[6] One of the earliest and most notable studies regarding the retention of motor skills was by Hill, Rejall, and Thorndike, who showed savings in relearning typing skills after a 25-year period with no practice.[2] Findings related to the retention of learned motor skills have been continuously replicated in studies, suggesting that through subsequent practice, motor learning is stored in the brain as memory. This is why performing skills such as riding a bike or driving a car are effortlessly and 'subconsciously' executed, even if someone had not performed these skills in a long period of time.[2]

Physiology

Motor behavior

When first learning a motor task, movement is often slow, stiff and easily disrupted without attention. With practice, execution of motor task becomes smoother, there is a decrease in limb stiffness, and muscle activity necessary to the task is performed without conscious effort.[7]

Muscle memory encoding

The neuroanatomy of memory is widespread throughout the brain; however, the pathways important to motor memory are separate from the medial temporal lobe pathways associated with declarative memory.[8] As with declarative memory, motor memory is theorized to have two stages: a short-term memory encoding stage, which is fragile and susceptible to damage, and a long-term memory consolidation stage, which is more stable.[9]

The memory encoding stage is often referred to as motor learning, and requires an increase in brain activity in motor areas as well as an increase in attention. Brain areas active during motor learning include the motor and somatosensory cortices; however, these areas of activation decrease once the motor skill is learned. The prefrontal and frontal cortices are also active during this stage due to the need for increased attention on the task being learned.[7]

The main area involved in motor learning is the cerebellum. Some models of cerebellar-dependent motor learning, in particular the Marr-Albus model, propose a single plasticity mechanism involving the cerebellar long-term depression (LTD) of the parallel fiber synapses onto Purkinje cells. These modifications in synapse activity would mediate motor input with motor outputs critical to inducing motor learning.[10] However, conflicting evidence suggests that a single plasticity mechanism is not sufficient and a multiple plasticity mechanism is needed to account for the storage of motor memories over time. Regardless of the mechanism, studies of cerebellar-dependent motor tasks show that cerebral cortical plasticity is crucial for motor learning, even if not necessarily for storage.[11]

The basal ganglia also play an important role in memory and learning, in particular in reference to stimulus-response associations and the formation of habits. The basal ganglia-cerebellar connections are thought to increase with time when learning a motor task.[12]

Muscle memory consolidation

Muscle memory consolidation involves the continuous evolution of neural processes after practicing a task has stopped. The exact mechanism of motor memory consolidation within the brain is controversial. However, most theories assume that there is a general redistribution of information across the brain from encoding to consolidation. Hebb's rule states that "synaptic connectivity changes as a function of repetitive firing." In this case, that would mean that the high amount of stimulation coming from practicing a movement would cause the repetition of firing in certain motor networks, presumably leading to an increase in the efficiency of exciting these motor networks over time.[11]

Though the exact location of muscle memory storage is not known, studies have suggested that it is the inter-regional connections that play the most important role in advancing motor memory encoding to consolidation, rather than decreases in overall regional activity. These studies have shown a weakened connection from the cerebellum to the primary motor area with practice, it is presumed, because of a decreased need for error correction from the cerebellum. However, the connection between the basal ganglia and the primary motor area is strengthened, suggesting the basal ganglia plays an important role in the motor memory consolidation process.[11]

Strength training and adaptations

When participating in any sport, new motor skills and movement combinations are frequently being used and repeated. All sports require some degree of strength, endurance training, and skilled reaching in order to be successful in the required tasks. Muscle memory related to strength training probably involves elements of both motor learning, described below, and long-lasting changes in the muscle tissue.

Evidence has shown that increases in strength occur well before muscle hypertrophy, and decreases in strength due to detraining or ceasing to repeat the exercise over an extended period of time precede muscle atrophy.[13] To be specific, strength training enhances motor neuron excitability and induces synaptogenesis, both of which would help in enhancing communication between the nervous system and the muscles themselves.[13]

A navy man performs strength training exercises.

However, neuromuscular efficacy is not altered within a two-week time period following cessation of the muscle usage; instead, it is merely the neuron's ability to excite the muscle that declines in correlation with the muscle's decrease in strength.[14] This confirms that muscle strength is first influenced by the inner neural circuitry, rather than by external physiological changes in the muscle size.

Previously untrained muscles acquire newly formed nuclei by fusion of satellite cells preceding the hypertrophy. Subsequent detraining leads to atrophy but no loss of myo-nuclei. The elevated number of nuclei in muscle fibers that had experienced a hypertrophic episode would provide a mechanism for muscle memory, explaining the long-lasting effects of training and the ease with which previously trained individuals are more easily retrained.[15]

On subsequent detraining, the fibers maintain an elevated number of nuclei that might provide resistance to atrophy; on retraining, a gain in size can be obtained by a moderate increase in the protein synthesis rate of each of these many nuclei, skipping the step of adding newly formed nuclei. This shortcut may contribute to the relative ease of retraining compared with the first training of individuals with no previous training history.[15]

Reorganization of motor maps within the cortex are not altered in either strength or endurance training. However, within the motor cortex, endurance induces angiogenesis within as little as three weeks to increase blood flow to the involved regions.[13] In addition, neurotropic factors within the motor cortex are upregulated in response to endurance training to promote neural survival.[13]

Skilled motor tasks have been divided into two distinct phases: a fast-learning phase, in which an optimal plan for performance is established, and a slow-learning phase, in which longer-term structural modifications are made on specific motor modules.[16] Even a small amount of training may be enough to induce neural processes that continue to evolve even after the training has stopped, which provides a potential basis for consolidation of the task. In addition, studying mice while they are learning a new complex reaching task, has found that "motor learning leads to rapid formation of dendritic spines (spinogenesis) in the motor cortex contralateral to the reaching forelimb".[17] However, motor cortex reorganization itself does not occur at a uniform rate across training periods. It has been suggested that the synaptogenesis and motor map reorganization merely represent the consolidation, and not the acquisition itself, of a specific motor task.[18] Furthermore, the degree of plasticity in various locations (namely motor cortex versus spinal cord) is dependent on the behavioural demands and nature of the task (i.e., skilled reaching versus strength training).[13]

Whether strength or endurance related, it is plausible that the majority of motor movements would require a skilled moving task of some form, whether it be maintaining proper form when paddling a canoe, or bench pressing a heavier weight. Endurance training assists the formation of these new neural representations within the motor cortex by up regulating neurotropic factors that could enhance the survival of the newer neural maps formed due to the skilled movement training.[13] Strength training results are seen in the spinal cord well before any physiological muscular adaptation is established through muscle hypertrophy or atrophy.[13] The results of endurance and strength training, and skilled reaching, therefore, combine to help each other maximize performance output.

Fine motor memory

Fine motor skills are often discussed in terms of transitive movements, which are those done when using tools (which could be as simple as a tooth brush or pencil).[19] Transitive movements have representations that become programmed to the premotor cortex, creating motor programs that result in the activation of the motor cortex and therefore the motor movements.[19] In a study testing the motor memory of patterned finger movements (a fine motor skill) it was found that retention of certain skills is susceptible to disruption if another task interferes with one's motor memory.[1] However, such susceptibility can be reduced with time. For example, if a finger pattern is learned and another finger pattern is learned six hours later, the first pattern will still be remembered. But attempting to learn two such patterns one immediately after the other could cause the first one to be forgotten.[1] Furthermore, the heavy use of computers by recent generations has had both positive and negative effects. One of the main positive effects is an enhancement of children's fine motor skills.[20] Repetitive behaviors, such as typing on a computer from a young age, can enhance such abilities. Therefore, children who learn to use computer keyboards at an early age could benefit from the early muscle memories.

Music memory

Bimanual synchronized finger movements play an essential role in piano playing.
Playing the piano requires complex actions

Fine motor skills are very important in playing musical instruments. It was found that muscle memory is relied on when playing the clarinet, specifically to help create special effects through certain tongue movements when blowing air into the instrument.[21]

Certain human behaviours, especially actions like the finger movements in musical performances, are very complex and require many interconnected neural networks where information can be transmitted across multiple brain regions.[22] It has been found that there are often functional differences in the brains of professional musicians, when compared to other individuals. This is thought to reflect the musician's innate ability, which may be fostered by an early exposure to musical training.[22] An example of this is bimanual synchronized finger movements, which play an essential role in piano playing. It is suggested that bimanual coordination can come only from years of bimanual training, where such actions become adaptations of the motor areas.[23] When comparing professional musicians to a control group in complex bimanual movements, professionals are found to use an extensive motor network much less than those non-professionals.[23] This is because professionals rely on a motor system that has increased efficiency, and, therefore, those less trained have a network that is more strongly activated.[23] It is implied that the untrained pianists have to invest more neuronal activity to have the same level of performance that is achieved by professionals.[23] This, yet again, is said to be a consequence of many years of motor training and experience that helps form a fine motor memory skill of musical performance.

It is often reported that, when a pianist hears a well-trained piece of music, synonymous fingering can be involuntarily triggered.[22] This implies that there is a coupling between the perception of music and the motor activity of those musically trained individuals.[22] Therefore, one's muscle memory in the context of music can easily be triggered when one hears certain familiar pieces. Overall, long-term musical fine motor training allows for complex actions to be performed at a lower level of movement control, monitoring, selection, attention, and timing.[23] This leaves room for musicians to focus attention synchronously elsewhere, such as on the artistic aspect of the performance, without having to consciously control one's fine motor actions.[23]

Puzzle cube memory

Erik Akkersdijk is solving a 3×3×3 Rubik's Cube in 10.50s.

Speed cubers often will use muscle memory in order to learn large numbers of algorithms quickly. It is quickly found that memorization purely of letters corresponding to moves on the cube is extremely difficult. The average beginner will try to do something like this; however, an advanced cuber can learn much more efficiently with muscle memory. A simple repetition of algorithms will create a long-term knowledge of it. This plays a role in major speedcubing methods such as Fridrich for the 3×3×3 Rubik's Cube and EG for the 2×2×2 Pocket cube.

Gross motor memory

Gross motor skills are concerned with the movement of large muscles, or major body movements, such as those involved in walking or kicking, and are associated with normal development.[24] The extent to which one exhibits gross motor skills depends largely on their muscle tone and the strength.[24] In a study looking at people with Down Syndrome, it was found that the pre-existing deficits, with regard to verbal-motor performance, limits the individual's transfer of gross motor skills following visual and verbal instruction to verbal instruction only.[25] The fact that the individuals could still exhibit two of the three original motor skills may have been a result of positive transfer in which previous exposure allows the individual to remember the motion, under the visual and verbal trial, and then later perform it under the verbal trial.[25]

Learning in childhood

The way in which a child learns a gross motor skill can affect how long it takes to consolidate it and be able to reproduce the movement. In a study with preschoolers, looking at the role of self-instruction on acquiring complex gross motor chains using ballet positions, it was found that the motor skills were better learned and remembered with the self-instruction procedure over the no-self-instruction procedure.[26] This suggests that the use of self-instruction will increase the speed with which a preschooler will learn and remember a gross motor skill. It was also found that, once the preschoolers learned and mastered the motor chain movements, they ceased the use of self-instruction. This suggests that the memory for the movements became strong enough that there was no longer a need for self-instruction and the movements could be reproduced without it.[26]

Affect of Alzheimer's disease

It has been suggested that consistent practice of a gross motor skill can help a patient with Alzheimer's disease learn and remember that skill. It was thought that the damage to the hippocampus may result in the need for a specific type of learning requirement.[27] A study was created to test this assumption in which the patients were trained to throw a bean bag at a target.[27] It was found that the Alzheimer's patients performed better on the task when learning occurred under constant training as opposed to variable. Also, it was found that gross motor memory in Alzheimer's patients was the same as that of healthy adults when learning occurs under constant practice.[27] This suggests that damage to the hippocampal system does not impair an Alzheimer's patient from retaining new gross motor skills, implying that motor memory for gross motor skills is stored elsewhere in the brain.

Impairment

It is difficult to display cases of "pure" motor memory impairment because the memory system is so widespread throughout the brain that damage is not often isolated to one specific type of memory. Likewise, diseases commonly associated with motor deficits, such as Huntington's and Parkinson's disease, have a wide variety of symptoms and associated brain damage that make it impossible to pinpoint whether or not motor memory is in fact impaired. Case studies have provided some examples of how motor memory has been implemented in patients with brain damage.

Consolidation deficit

A recent issue in motor memory is whether or not it consolidates in a manner similar to declarative memory, a process that involves an initial fragile learning period that eventually becomes stable and less susceptible to damage over time.[1] An example of stable motor memory consolidation in a patient with brain damage is the case of Clive Wearing. Clive has severe anterograde and retrograde amnesia owing to damage in his temporal lobes, frontal lobes, and hippocampi, which prevents him from storing any new memories and making him aware of only the present moment. However, Clive still retains access to his procedural memories, to be specific, the motor memories involved in playing the piano. This could be because motor memory is demonstrated through savings over several trials of learning, whereas declarative memory is demonstrated through recall of a single item.[1] This suggests that lesions in certain brain areas normally associated with declarative memory would not affect motor memory for a well-learned skill.

Dysgraphia for the alphabet

Case study: 54-year-old woman with known history of epilepsy

This patient was diagnosed with a pure form of dysgraphia of letters, meaning she had no other speech or reading impairments.[28] Her impairment was specific to letters in the alphabet. She was able to copy letters from the alphabet, but she was not able to write these letters.[28] She had previously been rated average on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale's vocabulary subtest for writing ability comparative to her age before her diagnosis.[28] Her writing impairment consisted of difficulty remembering motor movements associated with the letters she was supposed to write.[28] She was able to copy the letters, and also form images that were similar to the letters.[28] This suggests that dysgraphia for letters is a deficit related to motor memory.[28] Somehow there is a specific portion of the brain related to writing letters, which is dissociated from copying and drawing letter-like items.

Sensory motor amnesia

Sensory motor amnesia (SMA) occurs when the portions of the nervous system responsible for involuntary or automatic movement, such as the spinal cord, take indefinite persistent control of movements that are normally under the person's voluntary, conscious control. As described by Thomas Hanna in his book Somatics: Reawakening the Mind's Control of Movement, Flexibility, and Health, SMA develops by habituation and the repeated execution of a movement pattern.[29]

See also

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 Krakauer, J.W.; Shadmehr, R. (2006). "Consolidation of motor memory". Trends in Neurosciences 29: 58–64. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.10.003.
  2. 1 2 3 4 Lee, D.T., & Schmidt, A.R. (2005). Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioural Emphasis. (4th ed). Windsor, ON: Human Kinetics
  3. Adams, A.J. (1987). "Historical Review and Appraisal of Research on the Learning, Retention, and Transfer of Human Motor Skills". Psychological Bulletins 101 (1): 41–74. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.41.
  4. Celnik, P.; Classen, J.; Cohen, G.L.; Duque, J.; Mazzocchio, R.; Sawaki, L.; Stephan, K.; Ungerleider, L. (2005). "Formation of a Motor Memory by Action Observation". The Journal of Neuroscience 25 (41): 9339–9346. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2282-05.2005.
  5. 1 2 Flanagan, R.J.; Ghahramani, Z.; Wolpert, M.D. (2001). "Perspectives and Problems in Motor Learning". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5 (11): 487–494. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01773-3.
  6. Shanks, D.R.; St; John, M.F. (1994). "Characteristics of Dissociable Human Learning Systems". Behavioural and Brain Sciences 17 (3): 367–447. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00035032.
  7. 1 2 Shadmehr, R; Holcomb, HH (1997). "Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation". Science 277: 821–25. doi:10.1126/science.277.5327.821.
  8. Brashers-Krug, T; Shadmehr, R.; Bizzi, E. (1996). "Consolidation in human motor memory". Nature 382: 252–255. doi:10.1038/382252a0.
  9. Atwell, P.; Cooke, S.; Yeo, C. (2002). "Cerebellar function in consolidation of motor memory". Neuron 34: 1011–1020. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00719-5.
  10. Boyden, E.; Katoh, A.; Raymond, J. (2004). "Cerebellum-dependent learning: the role of multiple plasticity mechanisms". Annu. Rev. Neurosci 27: 581–609. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144238. PMID 15217344.
  11. 1 2 3 Ma, L.; et al. ",. (2010). Changes in regional activity are accompanied with changes in inter-regional connectivity during 4 weeks motor learning". Brain Res. 1318: 64–76. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.073.
  12. Packard, M.; Knowlton, B. (2002). "Learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia.". Annu. Rev. Neuroscience 25: 563–93.
  13. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Adkins, DeAnna L.; Boychuck, Jeffery (2006). "Motor training induces experience specific patterns of plasticity across motor cortex and spinal cord". Journal of Applied Physiology 101: 1776–1782. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00515.2006.
  14. Deschenes Michael, R.; Giles Jennifer, A. (2002). "Neural factors account for strength decrements observed after short-term muscle unloading". The American Journal of Physiology - Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 282: R578–R583. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.00386.2001.
  15. 1 2 Bruusgaard, J. C.; et al. (2010). "Myonuclei acquired by overload exercise precede hypertrophy and are not lost on detraining". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107 (34): 15111–15116. doi:10.1073/pnas.0913935107. PMID 20713720.
  16. Karni, Avi; Meyer, Gundela (1998). "The acquisition of skilled motor performance: Fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 95: 861–868. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.861.
  17. Xu, Tonghui; Perlik, Andrew J (2009). "Rapid formation and selective stabilisation of synapses for enduring motor memories". Nature 462: 915–20. doi:10.1038/nature08389.
  18. Kleim Jerrery, L.; Hogg Theresa, M. (2004). "Cortical Synaptogenesis and Motor Map Reorganization Occur during Late, But not Early, Phase of Motor Skill Learning". The Journal of Neuroscience 24: 629–633. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3440-03.2004.
  19. 1 2 Dowell, L. R.; Mahone, E. M.; Mostofsky, S. H. (2009). "Associations of postural knowledge and basic motor skill with dyspraxia in autism: Implication for abnormalities in distributed connectivity and motor learning". Neuropsychology 23: 563–570. doi:10.1037/a0015640.
  20. Straker, L.; Pollock, C.; Maslen, B. (2009). "Principles for the wise use of computers by children". Ergonomics 52: 1386–1401. doi:10.1080/00140130903067789.
  21. Fritz, C.; Wolfe, J. (2005). "How do clarinet players adjust the resonances of their vocal tracts for different playing effects?". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 118: 3306–3315. doi:10.1121/1.2041287.
  22. 1 2 3 4 Kim, D.; Shin, M.; Lee, K.; Chu, K.; Woo, S.; Kim, Y.; Song, E.; Lee, Jun; Park, S.; Roh, J. (2004). "Musical Training-Induced Functional Reorganization of the Adult Brain: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study on Amateur String Players". Human Brain Mapping 23: 188–199. doi:10.1002/hbm.20058.
  23. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Haslinger, B.; Erhard, P.; Altenmüller, E.; Hennenlotter, A.; Schwaiger, M.; von Einsiedel, H. G.; Rummeny, E.; Conrad, B.; Ceballos-Baumann, A. O. (2004). "Reduced Recruitment of Motor Association Areas During Bimanual Coordination in Concert Pianists". Human Brain Mapping 22: 206–215. doi:10.1002/hbm.20028.
  24. 1 2 "Gross motor Skills – What are Gross Motor Skills".
  25. 1 2 Meegan, S.; Maraj, B. K. V.; Weeks, D.; Chua, R. (2006). "Gross Motor Skill Acquisition in Adolescents With Down Syndrom". Down Syndrome and Practice 9 (3): 75–80. doi:10.3104/reports.298. PMID 16869378.
  26. 1 2 Vintere, P.; Hemmes, N. S.; Brown, B. L.; Poulson, C. L. (2004). "Gross-Motor Skill Acquisition by Preschool Dance Stoudents Under Self-Instruction Procedures". Journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis 37 (3): 305–322. doi:10.1901/jaba.2004.37-305. PMC 1284506. PMID 15529888.
  27. 1 2 3 Dick, M. B.; Shankle, R. W.; Beth, R. E.; Dick-Muehlke, C.; Cotman, C. W.; Kean, M. L. (1996). "Acquisition and long-term retention of a gross motor skill in Alzheimer's disease patients under constant and varied practice conditions". Journals of Gerontology: Series B Psychological sciences and social sciences 51B (2): 103–111. doi:10.1093/geronb/51B.2.P103.
  28. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Kapur, N.; Lawton, N. F. (1983). "Dysgraphia for Letters: a Form of Motor Memory Deficit?". Journal of Neurological Psychiatry 46 (6): 573–575. doi:10.1136/jnnp.46.6.573.
  29. Hanna, Thomas L. (1987). Somatics: Reawakening the Mind's Control of Movement, Flexibility, and Health. Da Capo Press. ISBN 0738209570.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Tuesday, May 03, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.