Speech error
A speech error, commonly referred to as a slip of the tongue[1] (Latin: lapsus linguae, or occasionally self-demonstratingly, lipsus languae), is a deviation (conscious or unconscious) from the apparently intended form of an utterance.[2] They can be subdivided into spontaneously and inadvertently produced speech errors and intentionally produced word-plays or puns. Another distinction can be drawn between production and comprehension errors. Errors in speech production and perception are also called performance errors.[3]
Speech errors are common among children, who have yet to refine their speech, and can frequently continue into adulthood. They sometimes lead to embarrassment and betrayal of the speaker's regional or ethnic origins. However, it is also common for them to enter the popular culture as a kind of linguistic "flavoring". Speech errors may be used intentionally for humorous effect, as with Spoonerisms.
Within the field of psycholinguistics, speech errors fall under the category of language production. Types of speech errors include: exchange errors, perseveration, anticipation, shift, substitution, blends, additions, and deletions. The study of speech errors contributes to the establishment/refinement of models of speech production.
Psycholinguistic explanations
Speech errors are made on an occasional basis by all speakers.[1] They occur more often when speakers are nervous, tired, anxious or intoxicated.[1] During live broadcasts on TV or on the radio, for example, nonprofessional speakers and even hosts often make speech errors because they are under stress.[1] Some speakers seem to be more prone to speech errors than others. For example, there is a certain connection between stuttering and speech errors.[4] Charles F. Hockett explains that "whenever a speaker feels some anxiety about possible lapse, he will be led to focus attention more than normally on what he has just said and on what he is just about to say. These are ideal breeding grounds for stuttering."[4] Another example of a “chronic sufferer” is Reverend William Archibald Spooner, whose peculiar speech may be caused by a cerebral dysfunction, but there is much evidence that he invented his famous speech errors (spoonerisms).[1]
An outdated explanation for the occurrence of speech errors is the one of Sigmund Freud, who assumed that speech errors are the result of an intrapsychic conflict of concurrent intentions.[1] “Virtually all speech errors [are] caused by the intrusion of repressed ideas from the unconscious into one’s conscious speech output”, Freud explained.[1] This gave rise to the expression Freudian slip. His theory was rejected because only a minority of speech errors were explainable by his theory.[1]
Psycholinguistic classification
There are few speech errors that clearly fall into only one category. The majority of speech errors can be interpreted in different ways and thus fall into more than one category.[5] For this reason, you are well advised to be skeptical about percentage figures for the different kinds of speech errors.[6] Moreover, the study of speech errors gave rise to different terminologies and different ways of classifying speech errors. Here is a collection of the main types:
Type | Definition | Example |
---|---|---|
Addition | "Additions add linguistic material."[1] | Target: We Error: We and I |
Anticipation | "A later segment takes the place of an earlier segment."[1] | Target: reading list Error: leading list |
Blends | Blends are a subcategory of lexical selection errors.[6] More than one item is being considered during speech production. Consequently, the two intended items fuse together.[1] | Target: person/people Error: perple |
Deletion | Deletions or omissions leave some linguistic material out.[1] | Target: unanimity of opinion Error: unamity of opinion |
Exchange | Exchanges are double shifts. Two linguistic units change places.[1] | Target: getting your nose remodeled Error: getting your model renosed |
Lexical selection error | The speaker has "problems with selecting the correct word".[6] | Target: tennis racquet Error: tennis bat |
Malapropism, classical | The speaker has the wrong beliefs about the meaning of a word. Consequently, he produces the intended word, which is semantically inadequate. Therefore, this is a competence error rather than a performance error. Malapropisms are named after a character from Richard B. Sheridan’s eighteenth-century play "The Rivals".[3] | Target:The flood damage was so bad they had to evacuate the city. Error: The flood damage was so bad they had to evaporate the city. |
Metathesis | "Switching of two sounds, each taking the place of the other."[3] | Target: pus pocket Error: pos pucket |
Morpheme-exchange error[6] | Morphemes change places. | Target: He has already packed two trunks. Error: He has already packs two trunked. |
Morpheme stranding | Morphemes remain in place but are attached to the wrong words.[7] | Target: He has already packed two trunks. Error: He has already trunked two packs. |
Omission | cf. deletions | Target: She can’t tell me. Error: She can tell me. |
Perseveration | "An earlier segment replaces a later item."[1] | Target: black boxes Error: black bloxes |
Shift | "One speech segment disappears from its appropriate location and appears somewhere else."[1] | Target: She decides to hit it. Error: She decide to hits it. |
Sound-exchange error | Two sounds switch places.[6] | Target: Night life [nait laif] Error: Knife light [naif lait] |
Spoonerism | A spoonerism is a kind of metathesis. Switching of initial sounds of two separate words.[3] They are named after Reverend William Archibald Spooner, who probably invented most of his famous spoonerisms.[6] | Target: I saw you light a fire. Error: I saw you fight a liar. |
Substitution | One segment is replaced by an intruder. The source of the intrusion is not in the sentence.[1] | Target: Where is my tennis racquet? Error: Where is my tennis bat? |
Word-exchange error | A word-exchange error is a subcategory of lexical selection errors.[6] Two words are switched. | Target: I must let the cat out of the house. Error: I must let the house out of the cat. |
Speech errors can affect different kinds of segments or linguistic units:
Segment | Example |
---|---|
Distinctive or phonetic features | Target: clear blue sky Error: glear plue sky (voicing) |
Phonemes or sounds | Target: ad hoc Error: odd hack |
Sequences of sounds | Target:spoon feeding Error: foon speeding |
Morphemes | Target: sure Error: unsure |
Words | Target: I hereby deputize you. Error: I hereby jeopardize you. |
Phrases | Target: The sun is shining./The sky is blue. Error: The sky is shining. |
Types
- Grammatical - For example, children take time to learn irregular verbs, so in English use the -ed form incorrectly. This is explored by Steven Pinker in his book Words and Rules.
- Mispronunciation
- Vocabulary - Young children make category approximations, using car for truck for example. This is known as hyponymy.
Examples
- "particuly" (particularly) ← elision
- "syntaxically" (syntactically) ← vocabulary
Scientific relevance
Speech production is a highly complex and extremely rapid process so that research into the involved mental mechanisms is very difficult.[6] Investigating the audible output of the speech production system is a way to understand these mental mechanisms. According to Gary S. Dell "the inner workings of a highly complex system are often revealed by the way in which the system breaks down".[6] Therefore, speech errors are of an explanatory value with regard to the nature of language and language production.[8]
Performance errors may provide the linguist with empirical evidence for linguistic theories and serve to test hypotheses about language and speech production models.[9] For that reason, the study of speech errors is significant for the construction of performance models and gives insight into language mechanisms.[9]
Evidence and insights
- Speech errors provide investigators with insights into the sequential order of language production processes.[6]
- Speech errors clue investigators in on the interactivity of language production modules.[8]
- The existence of lexical or phonemic exchange errors provides evidence that speakers typically engage in forward planning their utterances. It seems that before the speaker starts speaking the whole utterance is available.[6]
- Anticipation
- Target: Take my bike.
- Error: Bake my bike.
- Perseveration
- Target: He pulled a tantrum.
- Error: He pulled a pantrum.
- Performance errors supply evidence for the psychological existence of discrete linguistic units.
- Speech errors involve substitutions, shifts, additions and deletions of segments. "In order to move a sound, the speaker must think of it as a separate unit."[3] Obviously, one cannot account for speech errors without speaking of these discrete segments. They constitute the planning units of language production.[1] Among them are distinctive features, phonemes, morphemes, syllables, words and phrases. Victoria Fromkin points out that "many of the segments that change and move in speech errors are precisely those postulated by linguistic theories." Consequently, speech errors give evidence that these units are psychologically real.
- One can infer from speech errors that speakers adhere to a set of linguistic rules.
- "There is a complex set of rules which the language user follows when making use of these units."[3] Among them are for example phonetic constraints, which prescribe the possible sequences of sounds.[3] Moreover, the study of speech error confirmed the existence of rules that state how morphemes are to be pronounced or how they should be combined with other morphemes.[3] The following examples show that speech errors also observe these rules:
- Target: He likes to have his team rested. [rest+id]
- Error: He likes to have his rest teamed. [ti:m+d]
- Target: Both kids are sick. [kid+z]
- Error: Both sicks are kids. [sik+s]
- Here the past tense morpheme resp. the plural morpheme is phonologically conditioned, although the lemmas are exchanged. This proves that first the lemmas are inserted and then phonological conditioning takes place.
- Target: Don’t yell so loud! / Don’t shout so loud!
- Error: Don’t shell so loud!
- "Shout" and "yell" are both appropriate words in this context. Due to the pressure to continue speaking, the speaker has to make a quick decision which word should be selected.[4] This pressure leads to the speaker’s attempt to utter the two words simultaneously, which resulted in the creation of a blend.[4] According to Charles F. Hockett there are six possible blends of "shout" and "yell".[4] Why did the speaker choose "shell" and not one of the alternatives? The speaker obeyed unconscious linguistic rules because he selected the blend, which satisfied the linguistic demands of these rules the best.[4] Illegal non-words are for example instantaneously rejected.
- In conclusion, the rules which tell language users how to produce speech must also be part of our mental organization of language.[3]
- Substitution errors, for instance, reveal parts of the organization and structure of the mental lexicon.
- Target: My thesis is too long.
- Error: My thesis is too short.
- In case of substitution errors both segments mostly belong to the same category, which means for example that a noun is substituted for a noun. Lexical selection errors are based on semantic relations such as synonymy, antonymy or membership of the same lexical field.[2] For this reason the mental lexicon is structured in terms of semantic relationships.[3]
- Target: George’s wife
- Error: George’s life
- Target: fashion square
- Error: passion square
- Some substitution errors which are based on phonological similarities supply evidence that the mental lexicon is also organized in terms of sound.[3]
- Errors in speech are non-random. Linguists can elicit from the speech error data how speech errors are produced and which linguistic rules they adhere to. As a result, they are able to predict speech errors.
- Four generalizations about speech errors have been identified:[1]
- Interacting elements tend to come from a similar linguistic environment, which means that initial, middle, final segments interact with one another.
- Elements that interact with one another tend to be phonetically or semantically similar to one another. This means that consonants exchange with consonants and vowels with vowels.
- Slips are consistent with the phonological rules of the language.
- There are consistent stress patterns in speech errors. Predominantly, both interacting segments receive major or minor stress.
Information obtained from performance additions
An example of the information that can be obtained is the use of "um" or "uh" in a conversation.[10] These might be meaningful words that tell different things, one of which is to hold a place in the conversation so as not to be interrupted. There seems to be a hesitant stage and fluent stage that suggest speech has different levels of production. The pauses seem to occur between sentences, conjunctional points and before the first content word in a sentence. That suggests that a large part of speech production happens there.
Schachter et al. (1991) conducted an experiment to examine if the numbers of word choices affect pausing. They sat in on the lectures of 47 undergraduate professors from 10 different departments and calculated the number and times of filled pauses and unfilled pauses. They found significantly more pauses in the humanities departments as opposed to the natural sciences.[11] These findings suggest that the greater the number of word choices, the more frequent are the pauses, and hence the pauses serve to allow us time to choose our words.
Slips of the tongue are another form of "errors" that can help us understand the process of speech production better. Slips can happen at many levels, at the syntactic level, at the phrasal level, at the lexical semantic level, at the morphological level and at the phonological level and they can take more than one form like: additions, substations, deletion, exchange, anticipation, perseveration, shifts, and haplologies M.F. Garrett, (1975).[12] Slips are orderly because language production is orderly.
There are some biases shown through slips of the tongue. One kind is a lexical bias which shows that the slips people generate are more often actual words than random sound strings. Baars Motley and Mackay (1975) found that it was more common for people to turn two actual words to two other actual words than when they do not create real words.[13] This suggests that lexemes might overlap somewhat or be stored similarly.
A second kind is a semantic bias which shows a tendency for sound bias to create words that are semantically related to other words in the linguistic environment. Motley and Baars (1976) found that a word pair like "get one" will more likely slip to "wet gun" if the pair before it is "damp rifle". These results suggest that we are sensitive to how things are laid out semantically.[14]
See also
References
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 Carroll, David (1986). Psychology of language. Pacific Grove, CA, USA: Brooks/Cole Pub. Co. pp. 253–256. ISBN 0-534-05640-7. OCLC 12583436.
- 1 2 Bussmann, Hadumod. Routledge dictionary of language and linguistics. Routledge: London 1996, 449.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Tserdanelis, Georgios; Wai Sum Wong (2004). Language files: materials for an introduction to language & linguistics. Columbus: Ohio State University Press. pp. 320–324. ISBN 0-8142-0970-X. OCLC 54503589.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 Hockett, Charles F. (1973). "Where the tongue slips, there slip I". In Victoria Fromkin. Speech errors as linguistic evidence. The Hague: Mouton. pp. 97–114. OCLC 1009093.
- ↑ Pfau, Roland. Grammar as processor: a distributed morphology account of spontaneous speech. John Benjamins Publishing Co.: Amsterdam 2009, 10.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Eysenck, Michael W.; Keane, Mark A. (2005). Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Handbook. Psychology Press (UK). p. 402. ISBN 1-84169-359-6. OCLC 608153953.
- ↑ Anderson, John R. Kognitive Psychologie. Spektrum Akademischer Verlag: Heidelberg 1996 (2nd edition), 353.
- 1 2 Smith, Derek J. "Speech Errors, Speech Production Models, and Speech Pathology." Human Information Processing. Date of last revision: 12 December 2003. Date of access: 27th February2010. http://www.smithsrisca.demon.co.uk/speech-errors.html.
- 1 2 Fromkin, Victoria (1973). "Introduction". In Victoria Fromkin. Speech errors as linguistic evidence. The Hague: Mouton. p. 13. ISBN 90-279-2668-9. OCLC 1009093.
- ↑ Clark HH, Fox Tree JE (May 2002). "Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking". Cognition 84 (1): 73–111. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00017-3. PMID 12062148.
- ↑ Schachter, Stanley; Nicholas Christenfeld; Bernard Ravina; Frances Bilous (March 1991). "Speech Disfluency and the Structure of Knowledge". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60 (3): 362–367. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.362. Retrieved 2010-08-18.
- ↑ Garrett, M. F. (1975). "The analysis of sentence production.". In Gordon H Bower. The Psychology of learning and motivation. Volume 9 : advances in research and theory. New York: Academic Press,. pp. 133–177. ISBN 978-0-12-543309-9. OCLC 24672687.
- ↑ Baars,, Bernard J.; Michael T. Motley; Donald G. MacKay (August 1975). "Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 14 (4): 382–391. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(75)80017-X.
- ↑ Motley, Michael T.; Bernard J. Baars (1976). "Semantic bias effects on the outcomes of verbal slips". Cognition 43 (2): 177–187. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(76)90003-2. Retrieved 2010-08-19.
Further reading
- Bock, J. K. (1982). Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax. Psychological Review, 89, 1-47.
- Garrett, M. F. (1976). Syntactic processing in sentence production. In E. Walker & R. Wales (Eds.), New approaches to language mechanisms (pp. 231–256). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
- Garrett, M. F. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production: Vol. 1. Speech and talk (pp. 177–220). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
- Hickok G (2012). "The cortical organization of speech processing: feedback control and predictive coding the context of a dual-stream model". J Commun Disord 45 (6): 393–402. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2012.06.004. PMC 3468690. PMID 22766458.
- Jescheniak, J.D., Levelt, W.J.M (1994). Word Frequency Effects in Speech Production: Retrieval of Syntactic Information and of Phonological Form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Vol. 20, (pp. 824–843)
- Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Poeppel D, Emmorey K, Hickok G, Pylkkänen L (October 2012). "Towards a new neurobiology of language". J. Neurosci. 32 (41): 14125–31. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3244-12.2012. PMC 3495005. PMID 23055482.
- Reichman, R. (1981). Plain Speaking: A Theory and Grammar of Spontaneous Discourse. Cambridge, MA
- Bache, Richard Meade. (1869). Vulgarisms and Other Errors of Speech.
External links
Look up lapsus linguae in Wiktionary, the free dictionary. |