Stress and vowel reduction in English
Stress is a prominent feature of the English language, both at the level of the word (lexical stress) and at the level of the phrase or sentence (prosodic stress). Absence of stress on a syllable, or on a word in some cases, is frequently associated in English with vowel reduction – many such syllables are pronounced with a centralized vowel (schwa) or with certain other vowels that are described as being "reduced" (or sometimes with a syllabic consonant as the syllable nucleus rather than a vowel). Various phonological analyses exist for these phenomena.
Lexical and prosodic stress
Lexical stress (word stress) is regarded as being phonemic in English; the position of the stress is generally unpredictable and can serve to distinguish words. For example, the words insight and incite are distinguished in pronunciation only by the fact that the stress is placed on the first syllable in the former word, and on the second syllable in the latter. Similarly, the noun and the verb increase are distinguished by the placement of the stress in the same way – this is an example of an initial-stress-derived noun. Moreover, even within a given letter sequence and a given part of speech, lexical stress may distinguish between different words or between different meanings of the same word (depending on differences in theory about what constitutes a distinct word): For example, initial-stress pronunciations of offense and defense denote concepts specific to sports, whereas pronunciations with stress on the words' respective second syllables denote concepts related to the legal (and, for defense, the military) field and encountered in sports only as borrowed from the legal field in the context of adjudicating rules violations.
Some words are shown in dictionaries as having two levels of stress: primary and secondary. For example, the RP pronunciation of organization may be given as /ˌɔːɡənaɪˈzeɪʃən/, with primary stress on the fourth syllable, secondary stress on the first syllable, and the remaining syllables unstressed. For different ways of analysing levels of stress in English, see Degrees of lexical stress below.
English also has relatively strong prosodic stress, whereby particular words within a phrase or sentence are given additional stress in order to place emphasis on the information that they convey. There is also said to be a natural "tonic stress" which falls on the last stressed syllable of a prosodic unit – for more on this, see below under Descriptions with only one level of stress.
English is classed as a stress-timed language, which means that there is a tendency to speak so that the stressed syllables come at roughly equal intervals. See Isochrony.
Reduced vowels
Certain vowel sounds in English are associated strongly with absence of stress: they occur practically exclusively in unstressed syllables; and conversely, most (though not all) unstressed syllables contain one of these sounds. These are known as reduced vowels, and tend to be characterized by such features as shortness, laxness and central position. The exact set of reduced vowels depends on dialect and speaker; the principal ones are described in the sections below.
Schwa and r-colored schwa
Schwa is the most common reduced vowel in English.[1] It may be denoted orthographically by any of the vowel letters, as the a in about, the e in synthesis, the o in harmony, the u in medium, the i in decimal and the y in syringe (although the last two are pronounced as a near-close vowel by some speakers – see the following section).
In many rhotic dialects, an r-colored schwa, [ɚ], occurs in words such as water and standard (non-rhotic dialects simply have schwa in these positions, except where the dialect has linking R). The r-colored schwa can be analyzed phonemically as /ər/.
Reduced vowels in the close unrounded area
In some dialects of English there is a distinction between two vowel heights of reduced vowels: in addition to schwa, there is a distinct near-close central unrounded vowel [ɪ̈] (or equivalently [ɨ̞]). In the British phonetic tradition, the latter vowel is represented with the symbol /ɪ/, and in the American tradition /ɨ/.[2] An example of a minimal pair contrasting these two reduced vowels is Rosa’s vs. roses: the a in Rosa's is a schwa, while the e in roses (for speakers who make the distinction) is the near-close vowel. See weak vowel merger.
This vowel is sometimes informally referred to as schwi in analogy with schwa.[3]
The letter a is pronounced [ɪ̈] in many words, for example, message [ˈmɛsɪ̈dʒ], climate [ˈklaɪmɪ̈t], orange [ˈɔɹɪ̈ndʒ] etc.
Among speakers who make this distinction, the distributions of schwa and [ɪ̈] are quite variable, and in many cases the two are in free variation: the i in decimal, for example, may be pronounced with either sound. A symbolization convention recently introduced by Oxford University Press for some of their English dictionaries[4][5] uses the non-IPA "compound" symbol ⟨ᵻ⟩ (ɪ) in words that may be pronounced with either [ɪ̈] or schwa. For example, the word noted may be transcribed [ˈnəʊtᵻd].
The final vowel of words like happy and coffee is a reduced front close unrounded vowel most commonly represented with [i], although some dialects (including more traditional Received Pronunciation) may have [ɪ]. This [i] used to be identified with the phoneme /iː/, as in FLEECE.[6][7] See happy tensing. However, some contemporary accounts regard it as a symbol representing a close front vowel that is neither the vowel of KIT nor that of FLEECE; it occurs in contexts where the contrast between these vowels is neutralized;[8][9][10] these contexts include unstressed prevocalic position within the word, such as react [riˈækt]. For some speakers, however, there is a contrast between this vowel and /ɪ/ in such pairs as taxis vs. taxes and studied vs. studded. See English phonology – vowels in unstressed syllables.
Reduced vowels in the close rounded area
According to Bolinger (1986:347-360), there is a reduced rounded phoneme /ɵ/ as in willow /ˈwɪlɵ/, /ɵˈmɪʃən/, thus forming a three-way contrast with Willa /ˈwɪlə/ and Willie /ˈwɪlɨ/ or with a mission /ə ˈmɪʃən/ and emission /ɨˈmɪʃən/.
Analogously to the [ᵻ] symbol mentioned above, Oxford University Press have devised the non-IPA symbol [ᵿ] to represent a vowel which may be either /ʊ/ or /ə/, the two being in free variation.[11] For example awful [ˈɔːfᵿl] may be pronounced /ˈɔːfəl/ or /ˈɔːfʊl/. Phonologically, this vowel is an archiphoneme representing the neutralization of /ʊ/ and /ə/.
A rounded vowel [u], corresponding to the [i] happY vowel, is widely used in British works[12][13] for words such as influence [ɪnfluəns], into [ɪntu]. Phonologically, this vowel is an archiphoneme representing the neutralization of /uː/ and /ʊ/.
Syllabic consonants
The other sounds that can serve as the peak of reduced syllables are the syllabic consonants. When these occur, there is a syllable with no vowel. The consonants that can be syllabic in English are principally /l/, /m/ and /n/. For example, the le in cycle and bottle may be a syllabic /l/, the m in prism may be a syllabic /m/, and the on in prison may be a syllabic /n/.
A syllabic consonant may be analyzed phonologically either as just the consonant, or as consisting of an underlying schwa followed by the consonant. For example, cycle may be phonemized as either /ˈsaɪkl/ or /ˈsaɪkəl/.When a syllabic consonant occurs, an alternative pronunciation is also possible.[14] Like the le in cycle, some people will pronounce the schwa and the dark l sound while some of them will only pronounce the dark l.
A syllabic [ŋ] can also be heard in some positions, for example in the word and in the phrase lock and key, spoken rapidly (see Weak and strong forms of words below).
Unstressed full vowels
All full (unreduced) vowels may occur in unstressed position (except under theoretical approaches that routinely assign secondary or tertiary stress to syllables containing such vowels – see Degrees of lexical stress below). Some examples of words with unstressed syllables that are often pronounced with full vowels in Received Pronunciation are given below (pronunciation may be different in other varieties of English).
- Unreduced short vowels: /ɛ/ in the final syllable of document when used as a verb (compare the /ə/ heard when the word is used as a noun); /æ/ in the first syllable of ambition; /ɒ/ in the second syllable of neon; /ʌ/ in words with the negative prefix un-, such as unknown (compare /ə/ in until).
- Long vowels: /ɑː/ in the final syllable of grandma; /ɔː/ in the final syllable of outlaw; /uː/ in tofu; /ɜː/ in the noun convert; /iː/ in manatee. Note that this last may stand in contrast to the happY vowel found at the end of humanity. This contrast is further described under Distinctions between reduced and unreduced vowels below.
- Diphthongs: /eɪ/ in Monday; /əʊ/ in piano; /aʊ/ in discount; /aɪ/ in idea; /ɔɪ/ in royale.
Full vowels can often be found in unstressed syllables in compound words, as in bedsheet, moonlit, tentpeg, snowman, and kettledrum.[15] However, in some well-established compounds the vowel of the unstressed part may be reduced, as in postman /ˈpəʊstmən/.
Many other full unstressed vowels also derive historically from stressed vowels, due to shifts of stress over time (such as stress shifting away from the final syllable of French loan words, like ballet and bureau, in British English), or the loss or change of stress in compound words or phrases (as in óverseas vóyage from overséas or óverséas plus vóyage). There is a tendency, though, for such vowels to become reduced over time, especially in common words.
With vowels represented as /ɪ/ and /ʊ/, it may be hard to ascertain whether they represent a full vowel or a reduced vowel. An example of a word which may be considered to illustrate the contrast is chauvinism, where the first i may be regarded as an instance of the reduced vowel [ɨ], and the second as unreduced /ɪ/.
Degrees of lexical stress
Descriptions with primary and secondary stress
In many phonological approaches, and in many dictionaries, English is represented as having two levels of stress: primary and secondary. In every lexical word, and in some grammatical words, one syllable is identified as having primary stress, though in monosyllables the stress is not generally marked. In addition, longer words may have one or more syllables identified as having secondary stress. Syllables that have neither are called unstressed.
Secondary stress is frequently indicated in the following cases:
- In words where the primary stress falls on the third syllable or later. Here it is normal for secondary stress to be marked on one of the preceding syllables (which cannot normally be the one immediately preceding the primary stress). For example, interjection and evolution, which have their primary stress on the third syllable, consequently take secondary stress on their first syllables; organization and association both have primary stress on the fourth syllable, but have secondary stress on the first and second syllable respectively.
- Where the primary stress falls on the third or fourth syllable from the end, a following syllable may be marked with secondary stress.
- In many compound words, where one part of the compound is pronounced more prominently; here the stressed syllable of the prominent part of the compound is marked with primary stress, while the stressed syllable of the other part may be marked with secondary stress. For example, còunterintélligence [ˌkaʊntər.ɪnˈtɛlɪdʒəns], and cóunterfòil [ˈkaʊntərˌfɔɪl]. Dictionaries are not always consistent in this, particularly when the secondary stress would come after the primary – for instance the foil of counterfoil is transcribed with secondary stress in Merriam-Webster dictionaries but not in the OED, although both of them assign secondary stress to the counter of counterintelligence.
- In some dictionaries (particularly American ones), all syllables which contain a full (unreduced) vowel are ascribed at least secondary stress, particularly when they come after the primary stress (as in the counterfoil example above), although occasionally full vowels before the primary stress may also be marked for secondary stress. Bolinger (1986:358-360) notes that such dictionaries make use of the secondary-stress mark to distinguish full vowels from reduced vowels in unstressed syllables, as they may not have distinct symbols for reduced vowels. John Wells remarks, "Some analysts (particularly Americans) argue [...] that the presence of a strong [= full] vowel is sufficient evidence that the syllable in question is stressed. In the British tradition we regard them as unstressed."[15]
Note that this last-mentioned group of syllables are those which are ascribed tertiary stress in the approach described in the next section.
Descriptions with primary, secondary and tertiary stress
In some theories, English has been described as having three levels of stress: primary, secondary, and tertiary (in addition to the unstressed level, which in this approach may also be called quaternary stress). For example, our examples would be ²coun.ter.³in.¹tel.li.gence and ¹coun.ter.³foil. Exact treatments vary, but it is common for tertiary stress to be assigned to those syllables which, while not assigned primary or secondary stress, nonetheless contain full vowels (unreduced vowels, i.e. those which are not among the reduced vowels listed in the previous section).[16][17] Dictionaries do not generally mark tertiary stress, but as mentioned above, some of them treat all syllables with unreduced vowels as having at least secondary stress.
Descriptions with only one level of stress
Phoneticians such as Peter Ladefoged have noted that it is possible to describe English with only one degree of stress, as long as unstressed syllables are phonemically distinguished for vowel reduction.[18] According to this view, the posited multiple levels, whether primary–secondary or primary–secondary–tertiary, are mere phonetic detail and not true phonemic stress. They report that often the alleged secondary (or tertiary) stress in English is not characterized by the increase in respiratory activity normally associated with primary stress in English or with all stress in other languages. In their analysis, an English syllable may be either stressed or unstressed, and if unstressed, the vowel may be either full or reduced. This is all that is required for a phonemic treatment.
The difference between what is normally called primary and secondary stress, in this analysis, is explained by the observation that the last stressed syllable in a normal prosodic unit receives additional intonational or "tonic" stress. Since a word spoken in isolation, in citation form (as for example when a lexicographer determines which syllables are stressed) acquires this additional tonic stress, it may appear to be inherent in the word itself rather than derived from the utterance in which the word is spoken. (The tonic stress may also occur elsewhere than on the final stressed syllable, if the speaker uses contrasting or other prosody.)
This combination of lexical stress, phrase- or clause-final prosody, and the lexical reduction of some unstressed vowels, conspires to create the impression of multiple levels of stress. In Ladefoged's approach, our examples are transcribed phonemically as cóunterintélligence /ˈkaʊntər.ɪnˈtɛlɪdʒəns/, with two stressed syllables, and cóunterfoil /ˈkaʊntərfɔɪl/, with one. In citation form, or at the end of a prosodic unit (marked [‖]), extra stress appears from the utterance that is not inherent in the words themselves: cóunterintélligence [ˈkaʊntər.ɪnˈˈtɛlɪdʒəns‖] and cóunterfoil [ˈˈkaʊntərfɔɪl‖].
To determine where the actual lexical stress is in a word, one may try pronouncing the word in a phrase, with other words before and after it and without any pauses between them, to eliminate the effects of tonic stress: in the còunterintèlligence commúnity, for example, one can hear secondary (that is, lexical) stress on two syllables of counterintelligence, as the primary (tonic) stress has shifted to community.
Comparison
The following table summarizes the relationships between the aforementioned analyses of levels of stress in English: Ladefoged's binary account (which recognizes only one level of lexical stress), a quaternary account (which recognizes primary, secondary and tertiary stress), and typical dictionary approaches (which recognize primary and secondary stress, although their interpretations of secondary stress vary).
Description | Example | Binary approach | Quaternary approach | Dictionary approaches |
---|---|---|---|---|
The most prominent syllable when a word is spoken alone. | organization | Stressed | Primary stress | Primary stress |
Other phonetically prominent syllables in a word. | organization | Secondary stress | Secondary stress | |
Other syllables with unreduced vowels. | counterfoil | Unstressed | Tertiary stress | Secondary stress (esp. U.S.) or unstressed |
Syllables with reduced vowels. | counterfoil | Unstressed (quaternary stress) | Unstressed |
As described in the section above, the binary account explains the distinction observed between "primary" and "secondary" stress as resulting from the prosodic, tonic stress that naturally falls on the final stressed syllable in a unit. It also recognizes the distinction between unstressed syllables with full vowels, and unstressed syllables with reduced vowels, but considers this to be a difference involving vowel reduction and not one of stress.
Distinctions between reduced and unreduced vowels
As mentioned in the previous section, some linguists make a phonemic distinction between syllables that contain reduced vowels (as listed above – syllabic consonants are also included in this category), and those that, while being phonetically unstressed, nevertheless contain a full (unreduced) vowel. In some analyses syllables of the latter type are ascribed secondary stress (those of the former type being regarded as completely unstressed), while in others the reduced/unreduced distinction is regarded as one of vowel quality not involving any difference in stress. This last approach is taken by linguists such as Ladefoged[19] and Bolinger,[20] who thus consider that there are two "tiers" of vowels in English, full and reduced.
A distinction of this type may become useful for the analysis of a potential contrast between words such as humanity, chicory, shivery and manatee, chickaree, shivaree. When assuming a separate set of reduced vowels, the former may end with /ɨ/, while the latter may end with an unreduced /iː/. Another example, for some speakers, is provided by the words farrow and Pharaoh; the former may end with a reduced /ɵ/ while the latter may end with the unreduced /oʊ/.[21] Alternatively, these reduced vowels can be analyzed as instances of the same phonemes as full vowels. In that case, it may be the phonemic secondary stress that distinguishes these words.
Reduced vowel set | Secondary stress | No distinction | |
---|---|---|---|
shivery – shivaree | /ˈʃɪvərɨ – ˈʃɪvəriː/ | /ˈʃɪvəriː – ˈʃɪvəˌriː/ | /ˈʃɪvəriː/ (both) |
farrow – Pharaoh | /ˈfærɵ – ˈfæroʊ/ | /ˈfæroʊ – ˈfæˌroʊ/ | /ˈfæroʊ/ (both) |
Some linguists have observed phonetic consequences of vowel reduction that go beyond the pronunciation of the vowel itself. Bolinger (1986) observes that a preceding voiceless stop is likely to retain its aspiration before an unstressed full vowel, but not before a reduced vowel; and that flapping of /t/ and /d/ in American English is possible before a reduced vowel but not before a full vowel. Hence the /t/ in manatee would be an aspirated [tʰ], while that in humanity would be unaspirated [t] or a flap [ɾ].[22] Wells (1990) explains such phenomena by claiming that, in the absence of morpheme boundaries or phonotactical constraints, a consonant between a full and a reduced vowel generally belongs to the syllable with the full vowel, whereas a consonant between two reduced vowels belongs to the preceding syllable.[23] According to this analysis, manatee is /ˈmæn.ə.tiː/ and humanity is /hjʊ.ˈmæn.ɪt.i/; it is then asserted that voiceless stops are only aspirated at the beginning of syllables, and /t/ can only be flapped at the end of a syllable (as in might I /maɪt.aɪ/ → [mʌɪɾaɪ] versus my tie /maɪ.taɪ/ → [maɪtʰaɪ]).
Alternation between full and reduced vowels
It is a feature of English that reduced vowels frequently alternate with full vowels: a given word or morpheme may be pronounced with a reduced vowel in some instances and a full vowel in other instances, usually depending on the degree of stress (lexical or prosodic) given to it.
Alternation depending on lexical stress
When the stress pattern of words changes, the vowels in certain syllables may switch between full and reduced. For example, in photograph and photographic, where the first syllable has (at least secondary) stress and the second syllable is unstressed, the first o is pronounced with a full vowel (the diphthong of GOAT), and the second o with a reduced vowel (schwa). However, in photography and photographer, where the stress moves to the second syllable, the first syllable now contains schwa while the second syllable contains a full vowel (that of LOT).
Alternation depending on meaning
There are a number of English verb-adjective pairs that are distinguished solely by vowel reduction. For example, in some dialects, separate as a verb (as in 'what separates nation from nation') has a full final vowel, [ˈsɛpəreɪt], whereas the corresponding adjective (as in 'they sleep in separate rooms') has a reduced vowel: [ˈsɛpərət][24] or [ˈsɛprət].[25] A distinction may be made in a similar way between a verb and a noun, as in the case of document (pronounced with a schwa in the noun's final syllable and sometimes pronounced with a full vowel /ɛ/ in the verb's final syllable). Finally, differences in syllabic stress and vowel reduction (or lack of the latter) may distinguish between meanings even within a given part of speech, with the best-known such pairs in American English being offense and defense (in each case with the first syllable accented in the context of sports and the second syllable accented in legal contexts)
Alternation depending on type of enunciation
In some words, the reduction of a vowel depends on how quickly or carefully the speaker enunciates the word. For example, the o in obscene is commonly reduced to schwa, but in more careful enunciation it may also be pronounced as a full vowel (that of LOT). Compare this with the o in gallon, which is never a full vowel, no matter how carefully one enunciates.[22]
Weak and strong forms of words
Some monosyllabic English function words have a weak form with a reduced vowel, used when the word has no prosodic stress, and a phonemically distinct strong form with a full vowel, used when the word is stressed (and as the citation form or isolation form when a word is mentioned standing alone). In the case of many such words the strong form is also used when the word comes at the end of a sentence or phrase.
An example of such a word is the modal verb can. When appearing unstressed within a sentence and governing a verb (as in I can do it), the weak form /kən/ (or just /kn/, with a syllabic consonant n) is used. However the strong form /kæn/ is used:
- when the word is stressed: I don't have to do it, but I can do it
- when the word is phrase-final, i.e. without a governed verb: we won't be doing it, but they can if they want
- when the word is referred to in isolation: The verb "can" is one of the English modals.
In the case of most words with such alternative forms, the weak form is much more common (since it is relatively rare for function words to receive prosodic stress). This is particularly true of the English articles the, a, an, whose strong forms are used within normal sentences only on the rare occasions when definiteness or indefiniteness is being emphasized: Did you find the cat? I found a [eɪ] cat. (i.e. maybe not the one you were referring to). Notice that the weak form of the is typically [ði] before a vowel-initial word (the apple) but [ðə] before a consonant-initial word (the pear), although this distinction is being lost in the United States.[26] A similar distinction is sometimes made with to: to Oxford [tu] vs. to Cambridge [tə].
The exact set of words that have weak forms depends on dialect and speaker; the following is a list of the chief words of this type in Received Pronunciation:
- a, am, an, and, are, as, at, be, been, but, can, could, do, does, for, from, had, has, have, he, her, him, his, just, me, must, of, shall, she, should, some, than, that (as conjunction), the, them, there, to, us, was, we, were, who, would, you
In most of the above words the weak form contains schwa, or a syllabic consonant in the case of those ending /l/, /m/ or /n/. However, in be, he, me, she, we, been, him the vowel may be the reduced form of /ɪ/, or else [i]; and in do, who, you it may be the reduced form of /ʊ/, or [u]. (For the and to, see above.) These various sounds are described in the Reduced vowels section above.
Note that the weak form of that is used only for the conjunction or relative pronoun (I said that you can; The man that you saw), and not for the demonstrative pronoun or adjective (Put that down; I like that colour).
Another common word with a reduced form is our, but this is derived through smoothing rather than vowel reduction.
Other words that have weak forms in many varieties of English include your (weakly pronounced as [jə], or [jɚ] in rhotic accents), and my (pronounced [mɨ] or [mi]). These are sometimes given the eye dialect spellings yer and me.
In highly formal registers with exaggeratedly careful enunciation, weak forms may be avoided. An example is singing, where strong forms may be used almost exclusively, apart (normally) from a, although weak forms may be used more frequently as tempo increases and note-values shorten.
The vowel reduction in weak forms may be accompanied by other sound changes, such as h-dropping, consonant elision, and assimilation. For example, and may reduce to [ən] or just the syllabic consonant [n], or [ŋ] by assimilation with a following velar, as in lock and key. Compare also definite article reduction.
The 'em [əm] form of them is derived from the otherwise obsolete pronoun hem, an unusual form of suppletion.[27]
The homonymy resulting from the use of some of the weak forms can lead to confusion in writing; the identity of the weak forms of have and of sometimes leads to misspellings such as "would of", "could of", etc. for would have, could have, etc.
English weak forms are distinct from the clitic forms found in some languages, which are words fused with an adjacent word, as in Italian mangiarla, "to-eat-it".
See also
Notes
- ↑ Knight (2012), p. 71.
- ↑ Ladefoged (2006), p. 95.
- ↑ John David Ward, The Saga of Schwi, October 25, 2013.
- ↑ Upton, Kretzschmar & Konopka (2001), p. xiii.
- ↑ Key to Pronunciation. "Oxford English Dictionary". Oxford University Press.
- ↑ Wells (1982), p. 291.
- ↑ Wells (2012).
- ↑ Kreidler (2004), p. ?.
- ↑ McCully (2009), p. ?.
- ↑ Roach (2009), p. ?.
- ↑ Upton, Kretzschmar & Konopka (2001), p. xvii.
- ↑ Wells (2008), p. ?.
- ↑ Jones et al. (2011), p. ?.
- ↑ Knight (2012), p. 103.
- 1 2 John Wells, "strong and weak", in John Wells's phonetic blog, 25 March 2011
- ↑ Hirst & Di Cristo (1998), p. 57.
- ↑ Katalin (2008), p. 89.
- ↑ Ladefoged (2006), p. 83.
- ↑ Ladefoged (2006).
- ↑ Bolinger (1986).
- ↑ Bolinger (1986), p. 348.
- 1 2 Bolinger (1986), p. 358.
- ↑ Wells (1990), pp. 76–86.
- ↑ OED
- ↑ Macquarie Dictionary, Fourth Edition (2005). Melbourne, The Macquarie Library Pty Ltd. ISBN 1-876429-14-3
- ↑ Ladefoged (2006), p. ?.
- ↑ "'em". Oxford English Dictionary (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press. September 2005. (Subscription or UK public library membership required.)
Bibliography
- Bolinger, Dwight (1986), Intonation and Its Parts: Melody in Spoken English, Stanford University Press, ISBN 0-8047-1241-7
- Celce-Murcia, Marianne; Brinton, Donna M.; Goodwin, Janet M. (1996), Teaching Pronunciation: A Reference for Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages, Cambridge University Press, ISBN 0-521-40504-1
- Hirst; Di Cristo (1998), Intonation Systems: A Survey of Twenty Languages
- Jones, D.; Roach, Peter; Setter, J.; Esling, J. (2011), English Pronouncing Dictionary, 18th Ed., Cambridge University Press
- Katalin (2008), Beginner's English Dialectology
- Knight, Rachael-Anne Knight (2012), Phonetics- A course book, Cambridge University Press
- Kreidler, Charles (2004), The Pronunciation of English, Blackwell
- Ladefoged, Peter (2006), A Course in Phonetics, Thomson
- Garcia Lecumberri, M. Luisa (2000), English Transcription Course, Oxford University Press US, ISBN 0-340-75978-X
- McCully, C. (2009), The Sound Structure of English, Cambridge University Press
- Roach, Peter (2009), English Phonetics and Phonology, 4th Ed., Cambridge University Press
- Upton, C; Kretzschmar; Konopka (2001), Oxford Dictionary of Pronunciation for Current English, Oxford University Press
- Wells, John (1982), Accents of English, Cambridge University Press
- Wells, John C. (1990), "Syllabification and allophony", in Ramsaran, Susan, Studies in the pronunciation of English, pp. 76–86
- Wells, John C. (2008), Longman Pronunciation Dictionary, 3rd Ed., Longman
- Wells, John C. (2012-06-07), HappY Again, retrieved 2015-07-31
|