Affect (psychology)

Affect is the experience of feeling or emotion.[1] Affect is a key part of the process of an organism's interaction with stimuli. The word also refers sometimes to affect display, which is "a facial, vocal, or gestural behavior that serves as an indicator of affect" (APA 2006).

The affective domain represents one of the three divisions described in modern psychology: the cognitive, the conative, and the affective. Classically, these divisions have also been referred to as the "ABC of psychology", in that case using the terms "affect", "behavior", and "cognition". In certain views, the cognitive may be considered as a part of the affective, or the affective as a part of the cognitive.[2]

Affective states are psycho-physiological constructs. According to most current views, they vary along three principal dimensions: valence, arousal, and motivational intensity.[3] Valence is the subjective positive-to-negative evaluation of an experienced state. Emotional valence refers to the emotion’s consequences, emotion-eliciting circumstances, or subjective feelings or attitudes.[4] Arousal is objectively measurable as activation of the sympathetic nervous system, but can also be assessed subjectively via self-report. Arousal is a construct that is closely related to motivational intensity but they differ in that motivation necessarily implies action while arousal does not.[5] Motivational intensity refers to the impulsion to act.[6] It is the strength of an urge to move toward or away from a stimulus. Simply moving is not considered approach motivation without a motivational urge present.[7]

All three of these categories can be related to cognition when considering the construct of cognitive scope. Initially, it was thought that positive affects broadened cognitive scope whereas negative affects narrowed cognitive scope.[3] However, evidence now suggests that affects high in motivational intensity narrow cognitive scope whereas affects low in motivational intensity broaden cognitive scope. The cognitive scope has indeed proven to be a valuable construct in cognitive psychology.[3]

Theoretical perspective

"Affect" can mean an instinctual reaction to stimulation occurring before the typical cognitive processes considered necessary for the formation of a more complex emotion. Robert B. Zajonc asserts this reaction to stimuli is primary for human beings and that it is the dominant reaction for lower organisms. Zajonc suggests that affective reactions can occur without extensive perceptual and cognitive encoding and can be made sooner and with greater confidence than cognitive judgments (Zajonc, 1980).

Many theorists (e.g., Lazarus, 1982) consider affect to be post-cognitive: elicited only after a certain amount of cognitive processing of information has been accomplished. In this view, such affective reactions as liking, disliking, evaluation, or the experience of pleasure or displeasure each result from a different prior cognitive process that makes a variety of content discriminations and identifies features, examines them to find value, and weighs them according to their contributions (Brewin, 1989). Some scholars (e.g., Lerner and Keltner 2000) argue that affect can be both pre- and post-cognitive: initial emotional responses produce thoughts, which produce affect. In a further iteration, some scholars argue that affect is necessary for enabling more rational modes of cognition (e.g., Damasio 1994).

A divergence from a narrow reinforcement model of emotion allows other perspectives about how affect influences emotional development. Thus, temperament, cognitive development, socialization patterns, and the idiosyncrasies of one's family or subculture might interact in non-linear ways. For example, the temperament of a highly reactive/low self-soothing infant may "disproportionately" affect the process of emotion regulation in the early months of life (Griffiths, 1997).

Some other social sciences, such as geography or anthropology, have adopted the concept of affect during the last decade. In French psychoanalysis a major contribution to the field of affect comes from André Green.[8] The focus on affect has largely derived from the work of Deleuze and brought emotional and visceral concerns into such conventional discourses as those on geopolitics, urban life and material culture. Affect has also challenged methodologies of the social sciences by emphasizing somatic power over the idea of a removed objectivity and therefore has strong ties with the contemporary non-representational theory.

History

A number of experiments have been conducted in the study of social and psychological affective preferences (i.e., what people like or dislike). Specific research has been done on preferences, attitudes, impression formation, and decision making. This research contrasts findings with recognition memory (old-new judgments), allowing researchers to demonstrate reliable distinctions between the two. Affect-based judgments and cognitive processes have been examined with noted differences indicated, and some argue affect and cognition are under the control of separate and partially independent systems that can influence each other in a variety of ways (Zajonc, 1980). Both affect and cognition may constitute independent sources of effects within systems of information processing. Others suggest emotion is a result of an anticipated, experienced, or imagined outcome of an adaptational transaction between organism and environment, therefore cognitive appraisal processes are keys to the development and expression of an emotion (Lazarus, 1982).

Psychometric measurement

Main article: Affect measures

Affect has been found across cultures to comprise both positive and negative dimensions. The most commonly used measure of positive and negative affect in scholarly research is the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS).[9] The PANAS is a lexical measure developed in a North American setting and consisting of 20 single-word items, for instance excited, alert, determined for positive affect, and upset, guilty, and jittery for negative affect. However, some of the PANAS items have been found either to be redundant or to have ambiguous meanings to English speakers from non-North American cultures. As a result, an internationally reliable short-form, the I-PANAS-SF, has been developed and validated comprising two 5-item scales with internal reliability, cross-sample and cross-cultural factorial invariance, temporal stability, convergent and criterion-related validities.[10]

Mroczek and Kolarz have also developed another set of scales to measure positive and negative affect.[11] Each of the scales has 6 items. The scales have shown evidence of acceptable validity and reliability across cultures.[11][12][13]

Non-conscious affect and perception

Main articles: Unconscious mind and Perception

In relation to perception, a type of non-conscious affect may be separate from the cognitive processing of environmental stimuli. A monohierarchy of perception, affect and cognition considers the roles of arousal, attention tendencies, affective primacy (Zajonc, 1980), evolutionary constraints (Shepard, 1984; 1994), and covert perception (Weiskrantz, 1997) within the sensing and processing of preferences and discriminations. Emotions are complex chains of events triggered by certain stimuli. There is no way to completely describe an emotion by knowing only some of its components. Verbal reports of feelings are often inaccurate because people may not know exactly what they feel, or they may feel several different emotions at the same time. There are also situations that arise in which individuals attempt to hide their feelings, and there are some who believe that public and private events seldom coincide exactly, and that words for feelings are generally more ambiguous than are words for objects or events. Therefore, non-conscious emotions need to be measured by measures circumventing self-report such as the Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test (IPANAT; Quirin, Kazén, & Kuhl, 2009).

Affective responses, on the other hand, are more basic and may be less problematical in terms of assessment. Brewin has proposed two experiential processes that frame non-cognitive relations between various affective experiences: those that are prewired dispositions (i.e., non-conscious processes), able to "select from the total stimulus array those stimuli that are causally relevant, using such criteria as perceptual salience, spatiotemporal cues, and predictive value in relation to data stored in memory" (Brewin, 1989, p. 381), and those that are automatic (i.e., subconscious processes), characterized as "rapid, relatively inflexible and difficult to modify... (requiring) minimal attention to occur and... (capable of being) activated without intention or awareness" (1989 p. 381). But a note should be considered on the differences between affect and emotion.

Arousal

Main article: Arousal

Arousal is a basic physiological response to the presentation of stimuli. When this occurs, a non-conscious affective process takes the form of two control mechanisms; one mobilizing and the other immobilizing. Within the human brain, the amygdala regulates an instinctual reaction initiating this arousal process, either freezing the individual or accelerating mobilization.

The arousal response is illustrated in studies focused on reward systems that control food-seeking behavior (Balleine, 2005). Researchers have focused on learning processes and modulatory processes that are present while encoding and retrieving goal values. When an organism seeks food, the anticipation of reward based on environmental events becomes another influence on food seeking that is separate from the reward of food itself. Therefore, earning the reward and anticipating the reward are separate processes and both create an excitatory influence of reward-related cues. Both processes are dissociated at the level of the amygdala, and are functionally integrated within larger neural systems.

Motivational intensity and cognitive scope

Measuring Cognitive Scope Cognitive scope can be measured by tasks involving attention, perception, categorization, and memory. Some studies use a flanker attention task to figure out whether cognitive scope is broadened or narrowed. For example, using the letters “H” and “N” participants need to identify as quickly as possible the middle letter of 5 when all the letters are the same (e.g., “HHHHH”) and when the middle letter is different from the flanking letters (e.g., “HHNHH”).[14] Broadened cognitive scope would be indicated if reaction times differed greatly from when all the letters were the same compared to when the middle letter when different from the flanking letters.[14] Other studies use a Navon attention task to measure difference in cognitive scope. A large letter is composed of smaller letters, in most cases smaller “L”’s or “F”’s that make up the shape of the letter “T” or “H” or vice versa.[15] Broadened cognitive scope would be suggested by a faster reaction to name the larger letter, whereas narrowed cognitive scope would be suggested by a faster reaction to name the smaller letters within the larger letter.[15] A source monitoring paradigm can also be used to measure how much contextual information is perceived. In the source monitoring paradigm task participants watch a screen which serially displays words to be memorized for 3 seconds each and participants also have to remember whether the word appeared on the left half or the right half of the screen.[16] Participants are aware they will have to memorize the word and the location.[16] The words were also encased in a colored box, and the participants did not know that they would eventually be asked what color box the word appeared in.[16]

Main Research Findings Motivation intensity refers to the strength of urge to move toward or away from a particular stimulus.[17]

Anger and fear affective states, induced via film clips, conferred more selective attention on a flanker task compared to controls as indicated by reaction times that were not very different, even when the flanking letters were different from the middle target letter.[3][14] Both anger and fear have high motivational intensity because propulsion to act would be high in the face of an angry or fearful stimulus, like a screaming person or coiled snake. Affects high in motivational intensity, thus, narrow cognitive scope making people able to focus more on target information.[3][14] After seeing a sad picture, participants were faster to identify the larger letter in a Navon attention task, suggesting more global or broaden cognitive scope.[3][15] The sad emotion is thought to sometimes have low motivational intensity. But, after seeing a disgusting picture, participants were faster to identify the component letters, indicative of a localized more narrow cognitive scope.[3][15] Disgust has high motivational intensity. Affects high in motivational intensity, thus, narrow cognitive scope making people able to focus more on central information.[3][14][15] whereas affects low in motivational intensity broadened cognitive scope allowing for faster global interpretation.[15] The changes in cognitive scope associated with different affective states is evolutionarily adaptive because high motivational intensity affects elicited by stimuli that require movement and action should be focused on, in a phenomenon known as goal-directed behavior.[18] For example, in early times seeing a lion (fearful stimulus) probably elicited a negative but high motivational affective state (fear) in which the human being was propelled to run away. In this case the goal would be to avoid getting killed.

Moving beyond just negative affective states, researchers wanted to test whether or not the negative or positive affective states varied between high and low motivational intensity. To evaluate this theory, Harmon-Jones, Gable, and Price created an experiment using appetitive picture priming and the Navon task, which would allow them to measure the attentional scope with the detection of the Navon letters. The Navon task included a neutral affect comparison condition. Typically, neutral states cause broadened attention with a neutral stimulus.[19] They predicted that a broad attentional scope could cause a faster detection of global letters (large), whereas a narrow attentional scope could cause a faster detection of local letters (small). The evidence proved that the appetitive stimuli produced a narrowed attentional scope. The experimenters further increased the narrowed attentional scope in appetitive stimuli by telling participants they would be allowed to consume the desserts shown in the pictures. The results revealed that their hypothesis was correct in that the broad attentional scope led to quicker detection of global letters and the narrowed attentional scope led to quicker detection of local letters.

Researchers Bradley, Codispoti, Cuthbert and Lang, wanted to further examine the emotional reactions in picture priming. Instead of using an appetitive stimulus they used stimulus sets from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS). The image set includes various unpleasant pictures such as snakes, insects, attack scenes, accidents, illness, and loss. They predicted that the unpleasant picture would stimulate a defensive motivational intensity response, which would produce strong emotional arousal such as skin gland responses and cardiac deceleration.[20] Participants rated the pictures based on valence, arousal and dominance on the self-assessment manikin rating scale. The findings were consistent with the hypothesis and proved that emotion is organized motivationally by the intensity of activation in appetitive or defensive systems.[20]

Prior to research in 2013, Harmon-Jones and Gable performed an experiment to examine whether neural activation related with approach-motivation intensity (left frontal-central activity) would trigger the effect of appetitive stimuli on narrowed attention. They also tested whether individual dissimilarities in approach motivation are associated with attentional narrowing. In order to test the hypothesis, the researchers used the same Navon task with appetitive and neutral pictures in addition to having the participants indicate how long since they had last eaten in minutes. To examine the neural activation, the researchers used an electroencephalography and recorded eye movements in order to detect what regions of the brain were being used during approach motivation. The results supported the hypothesis suggesting that the left frontal-central hemisphere is relative for approach-motivational processes and narrowed attentional scope.[19] Some psychologists were concerned that the individuals who were hungry had an increase in the left frontal-central due to frustration. This statement was proved false because the research shows that the dessert pictures increase positive affect even in the hungry individuals.[19] The findings revealed that narrowed cognitive scope has the ability to assist us in goal accomplishment.

Clinical Applications

Later on, researchers connected motivational intensity to clinical applications and found that alcohol-related pictures caused narrowed attention for persons who had a strong motivation to consume alcohol. The researchers tested the participants by exposing them to alcohol and neutral pictures. After the picture was displayed on a screen, the participants finished a test evaluating attentional focus. The findings proved that exposure to alcohol-related pictures led to a narrowing of attentional focus to individuals who were motivated to use alcohol.[21] However, exposure to neutral pictures did not correlate with alcohol-related motivation to manipulate attentional focus. The Alcohol Myopia Theory (AMT) states that alcohol consumption reduces the amount of information available in memory, which also narrows attention so only the most proximal items or striking sources are encompassed in attentional scope.[21] This narrowed attention leads intoxicated persons to make more extreme decisions than they would when sober. Researchers provided evidence that substance-related stimuli capture the attention of individuals when they have high and intense motivation to consume the substance. Motivational intensity and cue-induced narrowing of attention has a unique role in shaping people’s initial decision to consume alcohol.[21] In 2013, psychologists from the University of Missouri investigated the connection between sport achievement orientation and alcohol outcomes. They asked varsity athletes to complete a Sport Orientation Questionnaire which measured their sport-related achievement orientation on three scales—competitiveness, win orientation, and goal orientation. The participants also completed assessments of alcohol use and alcohol-related problems. The results revealed that the goal orientation of the athletes were significantly associated with alcohol use but not alcohol-related problems.[22]

In terms of psychopathological implications and applications, college students showing depressive symptoms were better at retrieving seemingly “nonrelevant” contextual information from a source monitoring paradigm task.[16] Namely, the students with depressive symptoms were better at identifying the color of the box the word was in compared to nondepressed students.[16] Sadness (low motivational intensity) is usually associated with depression, so the more broad focus on contextual information of sadder students supports that that affects high in motivational intensity narrow cognitive scope whereas affects low in motivational intensity broaden cognitive scope.[3][16]

The Motivational Intensity theory states that the difficulty of a task combined with the importance of success determine the energy invested by an individual.[23] There are three main layers to the motivational intensity theory. The innermost layer says human behavior is guided by the desire to conserve as much energy as possible. This is simply stating that individuals aim to avoid wasting energy so they invest only the energy that is required to complete the task. The middle layer focuses on the difficulty of tasks combined with the importance of success and how this affects energy conservation. This layer focuses on energy investment in situations of clear and unclear task difficulty. The last layer looks at predictions for energy invested by a person when they have several possible options to choose at different task difficulties.[23] The person is free to choose among several possible options of task difficulty. The motivational intensity theory offers a logical and consistent framework for research. Researchers can predict a person’s actions by assuming effort refers to the energy investment. The motivational intensity theory is used to show how changes in goal attractiveness cause changes in energy investment.

Mood

Main article: Mood (psychology)

Mood, like emotion, is an affective state. However, an emotion tends to have a clear focus (i.e., its cause is self-evident), while mood tends to be more unfocused and diffused.[24] Mood, according to Batson, Shaw, and Oleson (1992), involves tone and intensity and a structured set of beliefs about general expectations of a future experience of pleasure or pain, or of positive or negative affect in the future. Unlike instant reactions that produce affect or emotion, and that change with expectations of future pleasure or pain, moods, being diffused and unfocused, and thus harder to cope with, can last for days, weeks, months, or even years (Schucman, 1975). Moods are hypothetical constructs depicting an individual's emotional state. Researchers typically infer the existence of moods from a variety of behavioral referents (Blechman, 1990).

Positive affect and negative affect (PANAS) represent independent domains of emotion in the general population, and positive affect is strongly linked to social interaction. Positive and negative daily events show independent relationships to subjective well-being, and positive affect is strongly linked to social activity. Recent research suggests that high functional support is related to higher levels of positive affect. In his work on negative affect arousal and white noise, Seidner found support for the existence of a negative affect arousal mechanism regarding the devaluation of speakers from other ethnic origins.[25] The exact process through which social support is linked to positive affect remains unclear. The process could derive from predictable, regularized social interaction, from leisure activities where the focus is on relaxation and positive mood, or from the enjoyment of shared activities. The techniques used to shift a negative mood to a positive one are called mood repair strategies.

Social interaction

Affect display is a critical facet of interpersonal communication. Evolutionary psychologists have advanced the hypothesis that hominids have evolved with sophisticated capability of reading affect displays.[26]

Emotions are portrayed as dynamic processes that mediate the individual's relation to a continually changing social environment.[27] In other words, emotions are considered to be processes of establishing, maintaining, or disrupting the relation between the organism and the environment on matters of significance to the person.[28]

Most social and psychological phenomena occur as the result of repeated interactions between multiple individuals over time. These interactions should be seen as a multiagent system—a system that contains multiple agents interacting with each other and/or with their environments over time. The outcomes of individual agents' behaviors are interdependent: Each agent’s ability to achieve its goals depends on not only what it does but also what other agents do.[29]

Emotions are one of the main sources for the interaction. Emotions of an individual influence the emotions, thoughts and behaviors of others; others' reactions can then influence their future interactions with the individual expressing the original emotion, as well as that individual's future emotions and behaviors. Emotion operates in cycles that can involve multiple people in a process of reciprocal influence.[30]

Affect, emotion, or feeling is displayed to others through facial expressions, hand gestures, posture, voice characteristics, and other physical manifestation. These affect displays vary between and within cultures and are displayed in various forms ranging from the most discrete of facial expressions to the most dramatic and prolific gestures.[31]

Observers are sensitive to agents' emotions, and are capable of recognizing the messages these emotions convey. They react to and draw inferences from an agent's emotions. It should be noted that the emotion an agent displays may not be an authentic reflection of his or her actual state (See also Emotional labor).

Agents' emotions can have effects on four broad sets of factors:

  1. Emotions of other persons
  2. Inferences of other persons
  3. Behaviors of other persons
  4. Interactions and relationships between the agent and other persons.

Emotions may affect not only the person at whom the emotion was directed, but also third parties who observe an agent's emotion. Moreover, emotions can affect larger social entities such as a group or a team. Emotions are a kind of message and therefore can influence the emotions, attributions and ensuing behaviors of others, potentially evoking a feedback process to the original agent.

Agents' feelings evoke feelings in others by two suggested distinct mechanisms:

People may not only react emotionally, but may also draw inferences about emotive agents such as the social status or power of an emotive agent, his competence and his credibility.[34] For example, an agent presumed to be angry may also be presumed to have high power.[35]

See also

References

  1. Hogg, M.A., Abrams, D., & Martin, G.N. (2010). Social cognition and attitudes. In Martin, G.N., Carlson, N.R., Buskist, W., (Ed.), Psychology (pp 646-677). Harlow: Pearson Education Limited.
  2. Duncan, S., & Barret, L.F. (2007). "Affect is a form of cognition: A neurobiological analysis". Cognition & Emotion 21 (6): 1184–1211. doi:10.1080/02699930701437931.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Harmon-Jones, E.; Gable, P. A.; Price, T. F. (5 August 2013). "Does Negative Affect Always Narrow and Positive Affect Always Broaden the Mind? Considering the Influence of Motivational Intensity on Cognitive Scope". Current Directions in Psychological Science 22 (4): 301–307. doi:10.1177/0963721413481353.
  4. Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C.; Amodio, D.M.; Gable, P.A. "Attitude toward emotions". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101 (6): 1332–1350. doi:10.1037/a0024951.
  5. Gable, P.A.; Harmon-Jones, E. "Does arousal per se account for the influence of appetitive stimuli on attentional scope and the late positive potential?". Psychophysiology 50 (4): 344–350. doi:10.1111/psyp.12023.
  6. "Emotion". The Penguin Dictionary of Psychology. Credo Reference: Penguin. 2009.
  7. Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C.; Price, T.F. "Which is approach motivation?". Emotion Review.
  8. Green, Andre (1973), The Fabric of Affect in the Psychoanalytic Discourse, The New Library of Psychoanalysis, London and NY, 1999
  9. Watson, D.; Clark, L. A.; Tellegen, A. (1988). "Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54 (6): 1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.
  10. Thompson, E. R. (2007). "Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS)". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 38 (2): 227–242. doi:10.1177/0022022106297301.
  11. 1 2 Mroczek, Daniel K.; Kolarz, Christian M. "The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75 (5): 1333–1349. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1333.
  12. Joshanloo, Mohsen; Bakhshi, Ali (2015-01-01). "The Factor Structure and Measurement Invariance of Positive and Negative Affect". European Journal of Psychological Assessment: 1–8. doi:10.1027/1015-5759/a000252. ISSN 1015-5759.
  13. Joshanloo, Mohsen. "Factor Structure of Subjective Well-Being in Iran". Journal of Personality Assessment: 1–9. doi:10.1080/00223891.2015.1117473.
  14. 1 2 3 4 5 Finucane, Anne M. (2011). "The effect of fear and anger on selective attention". Emotion 11 (4): 970–974. doi:10.1037/a0022574. PMID 21517166.
  15. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Gable, P.; Harmon-Jones, E. (14 January 2010). "The Blues Broaden, but the Nasty Narrows: Attentional Consequences of Negative Affects Low and High in Motivational Intensity". Psychological Science 21 (2): 211–215. doi:10.1177/0956797609359622.
  16. 1 2 3 4 5 6 von Hecker, Ulrich; Meiser, Thorsten (2005). "Defocused Attention in Depressed Mood: Evidence From Source Monitoring.". Emotion 5 (4): 456–463. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.456.
  17. Harmon-Jones, E.; Gable, P.A.; Price, T.F. (August 2013). "Does Negative affect always narrow and positive affect always broaden the mind? Considering the influence of motivational intensity on cognitive scope". Current Directions in Psychological Science 22 (4): 301–307. doi:10.1177/0963721413481353.
  18. Harmon-Jones, Eddie; Price, Tom F.; Gable, Philip A. (April 2012). "The Influence of Affective States on Cognitive Broadening/Narrowing: Considering the Importance of Motivational Intensity". Social and Personality Psychology Compass 6 (4): 314–327. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00432.x.
  19. 1 2 3 Harmon-Jones, Eddie; Gable, Phillip (April 2009). "Neural Activity Underlying the Effect of Approach-Motivated Positive Affect on Narrowed Attention". Psychological Science 20 (4): 406–409. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02302.x.
  20. 1 2 Bradley, Margaret; Maurizio Codispoti; Bruce Cuthbert; Peter Lang (September 2001). "Emotion and Motivation I: Defensive and Appetitive Reactions in Picture Processing". Emotion 1 (3): 276–298. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.1.3.276. PMID 12934687.
  21. 1 2 3 Hicks, J.A.; Friedman, R.S.; Gable, P.A.; Davis, W.E. (June 2001). "Interactive effects of approach motivational intensity and alcohol cues on the scope of perceptual attention". Addiction 107 (6): 1074–1080. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03781.x. PMID 22229816.
  22. Weaver, CC; Martens MP; Cadigan JM; Takamatsu SK; Treloar HR; Pederson ER (29 August 2013). "Sport-related Achievement Motivation And Alcohol Outcomes: An Athlete-Specific Risk Factor Among Intercollegiate Athletes". Addictive Behaviors 38 (12): 2930–6. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.08.021. PMID 24064192.
  23. 1 2 Richter, M. "A closer look into the multi-layer structure of Motivational Intensity theory". Social and Personality Psychology Compass 7 (1): 1–12. doi:10.1111/spc3.12007.
  24. Martin, Brett A. S. (2003). "The Influence of Gender on Mood Effects in Advertising" (PDF). Psychology and Marketing 20 (3): 249–273. doi:10.1002/mar.10070.
  25. Seidner, Stanley S. (1991). Negative Affect Arousal Reactions from Mexican and Puerto Rican Respondents. Washington, D.C.: ERIC.
  26. Nesse, R.M. (1990). "Evolutionary explanations of emotions". Human Nature 1 (3): 261–289. doi:10.1007/bf02733986.
  27. Keltner, D.; Haidt, J. (1999). "Social Functions of Emotions at Four Levels of Analysis". COGNITION AND EMOTION 13 (5): 505–521. doi:10.1080/026999399379168.
  28. Campos, J.; Campos, R. G.; Barrett, K. (1989). "Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation". Developmental Psychology 25 (3): 394–402. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.394.
  29. Smith, R.; Conrey, F.R. (2007). "Agent-Based Modeling: A New Approach for Theory Building in Social Psychology;". Personality and Social Psychology Review 11 (1): 87–104. doi:10.1177/1088868306294789.
  30. Rafaeli A. & Hareli S. (2007) Emotion cycles: On the social influence of emotion in organizations; Research in Organizational Behavior
  31. Ekman, P (1992). "An argument for basic emotion". Cognition and Emotion 6 (3/4): 169–200. doi:10.1080/02699939208411068.
  32. Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. 1994. Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  33. Rafaeli, A., Cheshin, A., & Israeli, R. (2007). Anger contagion and team performance. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Academy of Management, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.
  34. Frijda, N.H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  35. Tiedens, L. (2001). "Anger and advancement versus sadness and subjugation: The effect of negative emotion expression on social status conferral". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80 (1): 86–94. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.86. PMID 11195894.

Bibliography

External links

Look up affect or affective in Wiktionary, the free dictionary.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Thursday, April 14, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.