Culture and positive psychology

The interaction between culture and positive psychology is important to consider in order to better understand how positive psychology varies across cultures. Understanding these differences are vital in the formation and implementation of Positive Psychology interventions. More contemporary research accepts the notion of cultural sensitivity shown through efforts to understand and consider others. Culture influences if or how people seek help, how people define their social support structure, how people cope, as well as a variety of other factors relating to psychological well being.

Overview

Various cultural factors are thought to have an influence on the effects of positive psychology. These factors include transmitted patterns of ideas through values and beliefs, which are perpetuated through practices, institutions, and other channels, conceptions of the self and others, and views toward what constitutes the good life.

With the increasing amount of research being done, there are also increasing amounts of data showing that cultural factors do, in fact, affect people’s notions of perceived happiness. The current general literature discusses positive psychology into two categories: Western and Eastern. Cultural psychologist Richard Shweder argues that these factors help shape what people deem is good, moral, and virtuous. Westerners seek rewards on more of a physical plane, while Easterners seek to transcend the physical plane to a spiritual one.[1] While Western literature generally stresses autonomy, individuality, and personal satisfaction, their Eastern counterparts generally believe in harmony, collective cooperation, and group satisfaction.

More research is currently being done on their differences, as well as how they will affect applications of positive psychology. The increased knowledge on this topic will lead to a wider variety of techniques for developing healthier psyches in a global context. Namely, a greater knowledge of cultural aspects in positive psychology will aid professionals in many fields, such as medicine, education, and business. While there are controversies in the study of positive psychology in general, including large debate on how pervasive cultural attitudes affects a person’s perception of happiness, the large amount of interest in this field will generate more conclusive ideas in the future.

History

At first, research in positive psychology emphasized an individualistic framework, as most of the work in the field was done based in the United States. In the late 1800s and early 1900s, anthropologists and psychologists used race and culture as factors that influence positive and negative behaviors and attitudes. This originally led to a culturally deficient perspective, used for some groups to assert dominance over others through pseudoscience, eugenics, etc. American psychologists G. Stanley Hall and Henry Goddard Hall were among some of the notable figures to adopt this view. By the mid 20th century, psychologists realized that culture did not predetermine life outcomes; instead differences were the consequence of environmental factors. The culturally different perspective recognizes that unique strengths can be highlighted within every culture.[2]

As it became clear that culture accounts for differences between distinct groups and that culture influences people’s beliefs and values, researchers have considered the ways in which culture may impact people’s understandings of certain positive psychology constructs, like societal values. There is a historical basis for why certain qualities have become widely accepted virtues across different cultures. In a meta-analysis, Dahlsgaard, Peterson, and Seligman (2005)[3] traced the origins of the virtues courage, justice, humanity, temperance, wisdom, and transcendence to historical texts written by Confucianism, Taoism, Buddhism, Hindu, Christian, Islamic, Jewish, and from Athenian scholars. The works of these famous scholars reached many people worldwide and can be attributed to making these virtues well known and widely accepted as valuable qualities (Dahlsgaard, Peterson, & Seligman, 2005).

David Satcher, the 16th Surgeon General of the United States was among the first to emphasize the crucial influences of culture on mental health (see: Mental Health: Culture, Race, Ethnicity). Cultural differences occur both between and within nations. Social psychologists have supported the notion that as “social animals”.[4]

Major theories

Psychologists Charles R. Snyder, Shane J. Lopez, and Jennifer T. Pedrotti identify two major influential western traditions - Athenian and Judeo-Christian - and four major eastern traditions - Confucianism (China), Taoism (China), Buddhism (Japan), and Hinduism (Southeast Asia) to take into account.[5]

Western philosophies

  • Emphasizes the importance of a political community, or “polis
  • People with good human virtue arrange themselves into a society and model good behavior to develop human excellence
  • Faith, hope, charity, fortitude, justice, temperance, and wisdom
  • Laws and rituals serve to cultivate strengths within the tradition

Eastern philosophies

  • More emphasis in the west is placed on taking care of others within the group
  • Virtues are used to achieve enlightenment, or the good life
  • Tao is the energy surrounding and flowing through everyone
  • The Way must be achieved through experience, rather than teaching alone
  • There is, however, a possible end to the suffering through transcendence and ultimate understanding
  • Virtues are of utmost importance, and include love, joy, compassion, and composure
  • It advocates for the harmonious union among all individuals
  • Individuals are to strive for ultimate self betterment and also encouraged to be good to others
  • Good deeds are motivated by the process of karma

Major Cultural Differences in the Concepts of Well-being

Joshanloo (2014) identifies and discusses six broad differences between Western and non-Western conceptions of well-being. His analysis is based on his survey of the accounts of happiness and optimal functioning provided in Western as well as non-Western traditions, including Hinduism, Buddhism, Taoism, Confucianism, and Sufism. These six major domains of difference are as follows:[6]

Two Cultures Within Positive Psychology

Just as the impact and understanding of positive psychology is influenced by western and eastern philosophies, there are also different cultures within the field of positive psychology that guide the way people interpret positive psychology. With the support of numerous sources, Bacon (2005)[7] presents the idea that there are two “cultures” in positive psychology, or two different ways to view the positive psychology construct of personal strengths. He describes the focus culture as one where individuals are focused on developing and expressing their personal strengths. The second culture, balance culture, is instead oriented towards balancing and bringing harmony within oneself and among others. Bacon argues that individuals ascribing to the focus culture will differ in their life experience and life path than those who believe in the balance culture. Bacon believes that the strengths can be categorized into these two cultures and goes into depth explaining why creativity is the prototypical strength in the focus culture and why wisdom represents an ideal strength in the balance culture. Rather than tying culture to a certain ethnic or cultural group, Bacon and others argues that there are two different cultures, or schools of thought, within positive psychology. These two cultures reflect a new way to categorize strengths (Bacon, 2005).

Approaches

There are two main approaches to cross cultural positive psychology. The first, “culturally-free” perspective believes that there are numerous humans strengths that are valued universally, and that the pursuit of happiness is common across cultures. Proponents of this side view their social science as descriptive and objective, since its results “transcend particular cultures and politics and approach universality”.[8] The other approach views elements as culturally embedded, meaning that cultural values of the researchers influence daily work. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual 5 (DSM-V) embedded information throughout the manual to help increase cultural sensitivity and further the awareness of the need for a multicultural perspective in clinical practice. Some of the additional information included insight into cultural differences in key symptoms of disorders and suggestions on how to take the cultural context of the patient into account during treatment.[9]

The dominating framework establishes whether the reigning ideology of a region is individualistic or collectivistic. Cultural values affect what people consider to be strength versus a weakness. Western traditions fostered more individualistic societies while eastern traditions fostered more collectivistic societies. Individualistic cultures value the self above the group. Collectivistic cultures value the group above the individual.

Key western values (Individualism)

  • Autonomy/personal freedom
  • Competition
  • Personal achievement
  • Self-oriented
  • Future-oriented
  • Hope
  • Uniqueness (seen through commodities, names, attitudes, performances, attributes, etc.)

Key eastern values (Collectivism)

  • Seeks to cultivate interdependence
  • Sharing/cooperation
  • Conformity/desire to fit in (fosters duty to group and dependence)
  • Promote harmony/avoiding conflict
  • Going with the flow
  • Loyalty to family and friends [10]
  • Past-oriented
  • Group-oriented
  • Compassion

There are distinct differences between the thought processes used by Westerners and Easterners resulting in differing worldviews and approaches to meaning making. For example, in seeking happiness, Westerners give priority to “life, liberty, and the pursuit of happiness” as well as goal-directed thinking. Easterners, on the other hand, may be more accepting in their situation, and put more weight on inner life balance. The virtue of harmony appears to be a pillar of Eastern culture, and will warrant future research as time passes.

Edward C. Chang did a series of quantitative studies in order to showcase the importance of understanding the equivalence of traits and constructs across cultural groups. For example, Asian Americans were more pessimistic than Caucasians, but the two groups were not all that different on their levels of optimism.[11]

Ponterotto et al. have suggested that the ability to navigate and adapt to the increasingly diverse context of the world, is an important strength. People high on this are said to have “multicultural personalities”.[12] It adjusts for differences between cultures. Kristoffer G. van der Zee and Jan P. van Oudenhoven created the Multicultural Personality Questionnaire (MPQ), and identified five factors that describe personality style: cultural empathy, open-mindedness, emotional stability, initiative, and flexibility. This may correlate with enhanced well being.[13]

Psychology researcher Daphna Oyserman and her colleagues advocate for a less static view of separate cultures (East vs. West), and suggest exploring more dynamic ways explaining the ways in which these cultures operate. Different cultures can operate together, rather than being seen as conflicting (it is not “me vs. we”).[14]

Major empirical findings

There has been a recent effort by researchers to explore how concepts of Positive Psychology differ between cultures, as well as how culture influences how individuals view the good life.

For example, Snyder’s chapter (2009)[15] explores the way in which culture affects positive psychology. More specifically, Snyder acknowledges the fact that culture impacts individual’s understanding of strengths and weaknesses. Snyder claims that it is important to understand the cross section of positive psychology and culture, because it allows one to not only understand people within a culture but it also enables one to appreciate people from different cultures. Snyder outlines two schools of thought in the positive psychology field that pertain to how we should view strengths within a cultural context: culture-free and the culturally embedded perspective. Those ascribing to the culture-free perspective believe that strengths are not affected by culture and that there are universal strengths. The culturally embedded perspective argues that one must take into account culture when one is considering strengths, because they believe strengths manifest themselves differently in different cultures (Snyder, 2009).

Dahlsgaard, Peterson, and Seligman’s (2005)[3] meta-analysis presents historical and psychological evidence supporting the contention that there are universal virtues that exist across cultures. They are courage, justice, humanity, temperance, wisdom, and transcendence. These authors note that these virtues have been represented in ancient texts within Confucianism, Taoism, Buddhism, Hinduism, Christianity, Islam, Judaism, and from Athenian scholars. Each of these core virtues was represented in these schools of thoughts and became entrenched in the societies and cultures that ascribe to these religions and belief systems. Thus, these core virtues of courage, justice, humanity, temperance, wisdom, and transcendence are valued equally across these different cultures (Dahlsgaard et al., 2005).

Another virtue put forth by Seligman in attaining the good life is forgiveness. Previous researchers have not looked extensively at how forgiveness occurs in non-Western cultures, yet virtue is a very culturally embedded value. The research that has explored the virtue of forgiveness in non-Western cultures has found that there are not significant differences in overall levels of forgiveness, yet the reasons behind forgiving and the meaning of forgiveness varies between collectivist and individualistic cultures. For example, forgiveness in individualistic cultures is focused on a particular individual, while Eastern cultures see forgiveness in a broader, community context. Since forgiveness is a key virtue in positive psychology and a relevant topic in counseling, it is important to research forgiveness more thoroughly in a variety of cultures in order to better understand how different cultures view forgiveness.[16]

In studies that highlight differences between Western and Eastern cultures, discrepancies in values and emotions have been found when comparing the United States and China. It is often concluded that people in Eastern cultures are less happy than those in Western cultures, but research indicates that there is more value placed on suffering and transcendence in Eastern cultures than in the United States. It is also commonly believed that there is less happiness experienced by people in Eastern cultures because there is less individuality, but Eastern cultures are socialized in a much more collectivist mindset and develop into adults that are more concerned with finding a place in one’s community rather than standing out, as Americans are. These differences in particular indicate that there should be further research into cultural differences to avoid mistakes in classifying behavior and psychological functioning.

Kubokawa and Ottaway’s literature review examines how emotions vary across culture, suggesting that applications of positive psychology need to adapt to different cultural contexts. The authors use self-criticism as an example: self-criticism is considered as detrimental to one’s well-being in Western cultures, whereas collectivistic cultures value self-criticism as an important tool for personal growth, and consider it useful in aligning with societal norms (Heine et al., 2001). The authors also discuss a study conducted in Asian Americans and Caucasians that examined optimism and pessimism. The study found that while Asian Americans rated higher in pessimism than Caucasians, they did not differ in levels of depressive symptoms, indicating that the association between pessimism and depression might not apply in Asian cultures the way it does in Western cultures. In general, many negative emotions in Western cultures do not hold the same connotations in East Asian cultures, leading the authors to suggest that classification of certain emotions as positive and negative is not universal, and that research in positive psychology should cater to these cultural aspects.[17]

Kubokawa and Ottaway also present research that discredits the cultural relevance of the Values in Action Classification of Strengths (VIA) created by Peterson and Seligman. Peterson and Seligman (2004) identified six universal characters strengths and virtues that are valued by all cultures: courage, justice, humanity, temperance, wisdom, and transcendence. These virtues in turn led them to create a subset of 24 strengths common to all cultures. However, Christopher and Hickinbottom (2008) argue that the VIA Classification of Strengths is an oversimplification of cultures, and that while the values might be similar across cultures, the meaning that Peterson and Seligman ascribed to them are Western-orientated and might not translate well into more collectivist cultures.[17]

Cultural Differences and The Good Life

Previous research has demonstrated that the good life, as defined by subjective well-being, is rooted in predominantly Western ways of thinking. But, throughout history, subjective well-being has often become less vital in comparison to the needs to the larger community. This change results in individuals feeling subjective well-being in response to the success of their community rather than from their individual experiences. For example, the people of Bali see life as having two realms, everyday life and the spiritual world. Due to this dichotomy, it is difficult to examine subjective well-being without considering both realms, which occurs when positive psychology uses Western ways of thinking about the world.[18]

Another cultural difference in how one conceptualizes the good life can be seen in the beliefs of the Apsáalooke tribe, a Native American tribe in the US. Research has shown that for members of the Apsáalooke tribe, life satisfaction is deeply rooted in the belief that one’s life is intertwined with others in their tribe. Due to this feeling of collectivity, satisfaction is taken from helping others. This example is just one of many in which other cultures differ in the concept of satisfaction from the concept of satisfaction in an individualistic culture.[18]

In addition, a difference between collectivist and individualist cultures is the conceptualization of positive and negative emotions, including happiness. For example, research indicates that individualism moderates the relationship between hedonism and happiness, such that hedonism is more strongly related to happiness in more individualistic (vs collectivistic) cultures.[19] In individualist cultures, individuals attempt to avoid negative emotions, but in Eastern communities, some negative emotions are viewed as a virtue. One example of this is that collectivist cultures value shame because they view it as an opportunity to better themselves. So, when Western conceptualizations are applied to Eastern cultures, researchers run the risk of psychological imperialism.[18]

How does the culture of consumerism relate to positive psychology? Past research has shown that consumer culture and the pursuit of extrinsic goals leads to diminished well being, in comparison to pursuing intrinsic goals that lead to an increase in well-being. These findings not only occur in America, but the same results occurred in samples across a variety of countries, including Romania, Germany, Russia, Singapore, and South Korea. These countries represent both individualistic and collectivist cultures, which demonstrates that one possible universal in positive psychology across cultures is the importance of the pursuit of intrinsic goals.[20]

Cultural Differences in Well-being, Meaning, and Hope

Maygar-Moe, Owens, and Conoley identified specific cultural considerations that affect how practitioners should engage with concepts and theories in positive psychology within counseling settings. The literature review specifically addresses well-being, meaning, and hope.[21]

Well-being

Wealthy, individualistic cultures experience higher levels of social well-being than underprivileged, collectivistic cultures (Diener, Diener, & Diener, 1995). Self-esteem was also more predictive of life satisfaction in individualistic cultures than in collectivistic cultures (Suh, Diner, Oishi, & Triadis, 1998).[21]

Meaning

Research suggest that searching for meaning is viewed more positively in interdependent culture: Steger, Kawabata, Shimai, and Otake (2008) found that when comparing Japan (example of interdependent culture) and the U.S. (example of independent culture), the Japanese sample was high in searching for meaning while the U.S. sample was higher in experiencing meaning. Additionally, the search for meaning in the Japanese sample was positively related to presence of meaning, unlike in the U.S. sample.[21] Research also indicates that national levels of globalization can moderate the relationship between "thinking about the meaning of life" and life satisfaction. That is, in more globalized cultures, this relationship is negative, whereas in less globalized cultures, this correlation is positive.[22]

Hope

Maygar-Moe, Owens, and Conoley argue that while previous research supports the idea that is hope is universally a positive expectancy variably, the cultivation of hope varies based on cultural makeup.[21]

European Americans - life satisfaction serves as a source of agentic hope and positive affect was found to be predictive of pathways of hope. Therefore, European Americans would benefit most from interventions that improve life satisfaction and positive affect (Chang & Banks, 2007).

African Americans - lack of negative problem orientation was the strongest predictor for agentic, and positive problem orientation was the best predictor of pathways thinking. Therefore, African Americans would benefit best from interventions that simultaneously reduce a negative problem orientation and increase a positive problem orientation (Chang & Banks, 2007).[21]

Latinos - life satisfaction was the only predictor of pathways thinking, and rational problem solving was the best predictor of agentic thinking, therefore, Latinos would benefit most from interventions aiming to increasing rational problem solving and that would lead to higher life satisfaction (Chang & Banks, 2007).[21]

Asian Americans – positive problem orientation was the strongest predictor of pathways thinking, while positive affect was the strongest predictor of agentic thinking. Therefore, Asian Americans would benefit best from interventions that promote positive affect and a positive problem orientation (Chang & Banks, 2007).[21]

This illustrates how the application of different interventions within positive psychology would yield better result if they accounted for cultural differences.

Applications

Culturally appropriate psychoeducational and counseling interventions would benefit from more empirical research on culturally embedded positive psychology. Cross-cultural studies would help with the application of psychological treatment and recovery, along with improving the general understanding of the psyche of diverse populations of people. This is not only important for the differences between Western and Eastern civilizations, but has implications for the various cultural and ethnic groups within the United States of America, a society that has been considered a “melting pot” and has grappled with these issues throughout history. One important aspect of American culture that should be addressed is that of consumerism. Since the negative effects of the culture of consumption transcends specific cultures, it can become a common theme across counseling practices of positive psychology to encourage pursuing intrinsic values and the good life, and avoid pursuing extrinsic goals and the “goods” life.[20]

In addition, a culturally-embedded approach would allow professionals outside the mental health field to utilize methodology and concepts from psychology to motivate and help people. Specifically, employers in any field of business would be able to find techniques that are culturally appropriate for encouraging employees to be better engaged in their careers and to find meaning in their work. This can effectively be applied to the cultural network groups that many companies organize for employees, such as women’s and African-Americans’ networks, that allow for employees and employers to increase the understanding of perspectives and cultural sensitivity in the workplace.

Furthermore, teachers and administrators would be better equipped to address issues in educational achievement and behavioral development amongst diverse groups of students. Since there is currently strong criticism of the public education system for the achievement gap amongst students based on race and socioeconomic status, it would be especially helpful for educational authorities to understand the disparities that students may face due to aspects of their background. Teachers would benefit from knowing how to better foster a love of learning, creativity, optimism, resilience, leadership, and teamwork in diverse groups of students in order to prepare students for achievement in the future. The concept of achievement is tied to the aforementioned topics for many children. When achievement seems possible for all students, regardless of their cultural backgrounds, they may be more likely to persistently pursue attaining it.

Applications of Strengths Theory Across Cultures

Virtues and strengths are valued differently across cultures, which in turn means that attempts to build strengths using positive psychology within counseling settings can only be successful if they include important cultural considerations. For example, Chang (1996) found that Asian Americans reported higher levels of pessimism than Caucasians, however within that population pessimism was not found to contribute to depression and was associated with positive problem-solving strategies. This suggests that while pessimism is considered a weakness in some cultures, it might actually serve as a strength in others: defensive pessimism, for example, leads people to set lower expectations for themselves. This supports the idea that applications of positive psychology to cultivate strengths need to be altered to fit the clients’ cultural backgrounds.[21]

Controversies

The major problems with positive psychology and emotional research in general consists of four parts: Limitations of cross-sectional research, a widespread tendency to draw an over simple approach in drawing conclusions with emotions, overstatement of the importance of sample and cohort differences, and the use of overly casual procedures to measure emotion.

Limits of cross-sectional research

While there are no ideal research designs, most positive psychology research is done as a cross-sectional study, often administered through surveys. However, results from cross-sectional designs are cannot establish causality because it does not permit prediction from one time or circumstance to a different time. Also, most intra-individual and inter-individual studies show contradictory results. Therefore, most research conclusions are open to debate.

Oversimplification of conclusions

Generally, emotions are regarded as positive when a) the person feels good subjectively, b) they are brought about by life conditions, and c) when they result in a positive outcome. When a person feels a positive emotion (or reports it) it does not reflect the social context in which that person feels the particular emotion. Secondly, focusing on positive and negative emotions ignore the benefits of studying discrete emotions. “collapsing several discrete emotions into positive and negative is unwise and regressive”. Third, not all emotions can be discretely classified as either positive or negative. All emotions have a potential to be either negative or positive, such as hope, joy, pride, and anger.

Overstatement of sample and cohort differences

Individual differences in the way people experience positive and negative emotions affect study results in a way that makes sample and cohort differences less important than the studies stress. More specifically, the conception of the self is key in positive psychology, and cross cultural differences in the conception of the self make it difficult to generalize results.[18] Those individual differences often account for much of the variations between groups, and are often not taken into account.

Nonstandardized measurement of emotion

The first two problems converge for an even larger issue: the measurement of emotion is far from accurate. However, many studies and researchers use “oversimple checklists and questionnaires without follow-up or exploration of their adequacy.” Without in-depth explorations, the possibilities of finding observations about the flow of events in an emotional encounter is minimal. Thus, with very subjective results and the inadequacy to rule out other causal connections, the measurement of positive emotion itself is flawed.

With the cultural aspect of positive psychology, the problems largely consist of the definition of positive emotions and notions of a positive life. Many of the ideals that are associated with a positive psychology are notions that are deeply ingrained within Western cultures, and do not necessarily apply to all groups of people. In relation to the previous point about measuring positive emotion, many of the social conditions in emotion measurement are ignored.

"Positive psychology is doomed to being narrow and ethnocentric as long as its researchers remain unaware of the cultural assumptions underlying their work.”[17] A large part of the literature debates whether positive psychology is innately culture-free or culture-embedded. Those who advocate culture-free positive psychology state that happiness is a universal trait, whereas advocates of culture-embedded positive psychology believe that the cultural context reach happiness differently, depending on their culture.

Nonstandardized measurements of emotion are also due to the fact that pleasant hedonic experiences are expressed in very different ways across cultural groups, hindering researchers’ ability to choose universal terms that will accurately describe these experiences without completely disregarding their cultural context. For example, Mesquita and Frijda (1992) argue that the word for “happiness” not only changes across languages, it also describes a different emotional experience. In English, happiness refers to a “high arousal, exuberant experience,” while its equivalent in Hindi, sukhi, refers to a low-arousal experience of peace and happiness, and in Kenya, for the Kipsigis, “happiness” is a lack of negative experiences, indicating a quiet and calm state. This makes it challenging for researchers to study positive psychology across cultures, as different interpretations of these terms could lead to invalid assumptions about specific emotions.[23]

Future Research

Specifically in the field of positive psychology, future studies would improve understanding of subjective well-being among other topics if globalization was taken into consideration. As researchers aim to be more culturally competent when designing their studies, special attention would have to be paid to the significance of language, access to education, exposure to media, and more in order to ensure authenticity in the understanding of behaviors observed and responses received from sample populations.

References

  1. Shweder, R.A., Much, N.C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The "Big Three" of Morality (Autonomy, Community, Divinity) and the "Big Three" Explanations of Suffering. In A. Brandt & P. Rozin (Eds.) Morality and Health. New York: Routledge
  2. Sue, Derald Wing; Sue, David (4 May 2011). Counseling the Culturally Diverse: Theory and Practice. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-1-118-04489-6.
  3. 1 2 Dahlsgaard, K.; Peterson, C.; Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). "Shared virtue: The convergence of valued human strengths across culture and history". Review of General Psychology 9 (3): 203–213. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.9.3.203.
  4. Elliot Aronson (25 July 2003). The Social Animal. Worth Publishers. ISBN 978-0-7167-5715-3.
  5. C. R. Snyder (14 September 2010). Positive Psychology: The Scientific and Practical Explorations of Human Strengths. SAGE Publications. ISBN 978-1-4129-9062-2.
  6. Joshanloo, Mohsen (21 March 2013). "Eastern Conceptualizations of Happiness: Fundamental Differences with Western Views". Journal of Happiness Studies 15 (2): 475–493. doi:10.1007/s10902-013-9431-1.
  7. Bacon, S. F. (2005). "Positive psychology's two cultures". Review of General Psychology 9 (2): 181–192. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.181.
  8. Seligman ME, Csikszentmihalyi M (January 2000). "Positive psychology. An introduction". The American Psychologist 55 (1): 5–14. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5. PMID 11392865. Retrieved 2014-06-23.
  9. "Cultural Concepts in DSM-5" (PDF). DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association.
  10. Wong, Paul.T.P. (Oct 2004). "The loyalty factor: Key to the good life". International Network on Personal Meaning. Retrieved 23 June 2014.
  11. Chang, Edward C. (1996). "Evidence for the cultural specificity of pessimism in Asians vs Caucasians: a test of a general negativity hypothesis". Personality and Individual Differences 21 (5): 819–822. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(96)00110-9. ISSN 0191-8869.
  12. Joseph G. Ponterotto; Shawn O. Utsey; Paul B. Pedersen (28 March 2006). Preventing Prejudice: A Guide for Counselors, Educators, and Parents. SAGE Publications. ISBN 978-0-7619-2818-8.
  13. van der Zee K, van Oudenhoven JP, Ponterotto JG, Fietzer AW (2013). "Multicultural personality questionnaire: development of a short form.". J Pers Assess 95 (1): 118–24. doi:10.1080/00223891.2012.718302. PMID 22966866.
  14. Oyserman D, Lee SW (2008). "Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism.". Psychol Bull 134 (2): 311–42. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311. PMID 18298274.
  15. Snyder, C. (2009). Positive Psychology Within a Cultural Context. In Oxford handbook of positive psychology (pp. 49-57). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  16. Sandage, S.J.; Hill, P.C.; Vang, H.C. (2003). "Toward a multicultural positive psychology: Indigenous forgiveness and Hmong culture". The Counseling Psychologist 31 (5): 564–592. doi:10.1177/0011000003256350.
  17. 1 2 3 Kubokawa, A. and Ottaway, A. (2009). Positive Psychology and Cultural Sensitivity: A Review of the Literature. Graduate Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1(2), 130-138
  18. 1 2 3 4 Christopher, J.C.; Hickinbottom, S. (2008). "Positive psychology, ethnocentrism, and the disguised ideology of individualism". Theory & Psychology 18 (5): 563–589. doi:10.1177/0959354308093396.
  19. Joshanloo, Mohsen; Jarden, Aaron (2016-05-01). "Individualism as the moderator of the relationship between hedonism and happiness: A study in 19 nations". Personality and Individual Differences 94: 149–152. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.01.025.
  20. 1 2 Kasser, T (2004). Positive Psychology in Practice (1 ed.). Wiley. pp. 55–65. ISBN 978-0471459064.
  21. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Magyar-Moe, J. L.; Owens, R. L.; Conoley, C. W. (2015). "Positive Psychological Interventions in Counseling: What Every Counseling Psychologist Should Know". The Counseling Psychologist 1 (2): 1–50. doi:10.1177/0011000015573776.
  22. Joshanloo, Mohsen; Weijers, Dan (2 January 2014). "Does thinking about the meaning of life make you happy in a religious and globalised world? A 75-nation study". Journal of Psychology in Africa 24 (1): 73–81. doi:10.1080/14330237.2014.904093. ISSN 1433-0237.
  23. Mitamura, C., Leu, J., Campos, B., Boccagno, C., and Tugade, M. M. (2014). Traversing Affective Boundaries: examining Cultural Norms for Positive Emotions. In Tugade, M. M., Shiota, M. N., and Kirby, L. D. (Eds.), Handbook of Positive Emotions (pp. 229-240). New York: The Guilford Press.

Further reading

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Saturday, April 16, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.