Experimental analysis of behavior
The experimental analysis of behavior (EAB) is the name given to the school of psychology founded on B. F. Skinner’s philosophy of radical behaviorism. A central principle was the inductive, data-driven[1] examination of functional relations, as opposed to the kinds of hypothetico-deductive learning theory[2] that had grown up in the comparative psychology of the 1920–1950 period. Skinner's approach was characterized by empirical observation of measurable behavior which could be predicted and controlled. It owed its early success to the effectiveness of Skinner's procedures of operant conditioning, both in the laboratory and in behavior therapy—what is now called applied behavior analysis
Basic learning processes in behavior analysis
Classical (or respondent) conditioning
In classical or respondent conditioning a relatively neutral stimulus (conditioned stimulus) comes to signal the occurrence of a biologically significant stimulus (unconditioned stimulus) such as food or pain. This typically done by repeatedly pairing the two stimuli, as in Pavlov's experiments with dogs, where a bell was followed by food. As a result, the conditioned stimulus yields a conditioned response that is usually similar to the unconditioned response elicited by unconditioned stimulus. (e.g. salivation, in Pavlov's dogs.) [3]
Operant (or instrumental) conditioning
Operant conditioning (also, “instrumental conditioning”) is a learning process in which behavior is sensitive to, or controlled by its consequences. For example, behavior that is followed by reward (reinforcement) becomes more probable, whereas behavior that is followed by punishment becomes less probable.[4] Many variations and details of this process may be found in the main article.
Experimental tools in behavioral research
Operant conditioning chamber
The most commonly used tool in animal behavioral research is the operant conditioning chamber—also known as a Skinner Box. The chamber is an enclosure designed to hold a test animal (often a rodent, pigeon, or primate). The interior of the chamber contains some type of device that serves the role of discriminative stimuli, at least one mechanism to measure the subject's behavior as a rate of response—such as a lever or key-peck switch—and a mechanism for the delivery of consequences—such as a food pellet dispenser or a token reinforcer such as an LED light.
Cumulative recorder
Of historical interest is the cumulative recorder, an instrument used to record the responses of subjects graphically. Traditionally, its graphing mechanism has consisted of a rotating drum of paper equipped with a marking needle. The needle would start at the bottom of the page and the drum would turn the roll of paper horizontally. Each subject response would result in the marking needle moving vertically along the paper one tick. This makes it possible for the rate of response to be calculated by finding the slope of the graph at a given point. For example, a regular rate of response would cause the needle to move vertically at a regular rate, resulting in a straight diagonal line rising towards the right. An accelerating or decelerating rate of response would lead to a quadratic (or similar) curve. For the most part, cumulative records are no longer graphed using rotating drums, but are recorded electronically instead.
Philosophical basis of behavioral research
Laboratory methods employed in the experimental analysis of behavior are based upon B.F. Skinner's philosophy of radical behaviorism, which is premised upon:
- Everything that organisms do is behavior (including thinking), and
- All behavior is lawful, which allows itself to be experimentally studied.
Central to behavior analysis is the use of a Four-Term Contingency (Motivating Operation, Discriminative Stimulus, Response, Reinforcing Stimulus) to describe functional relationships in the control of behavior.
- Motivating operations (MO) that establish whether or not an organism's behavior will be affected by a consequence. Such antecedent states include establishing operations, which increase the effectiveness of their corresponding consequences, and abolishing operations, which decrease their effectiveness. Besides modulating the effectiveness of consequences, these motivating operations can also acquire discriminative control over an organism's behavior.[5] For example, an organism which is in a state of hunger (establishes food as an effective reinforcer) is more likely to engage in behavior that has previously resulted in being fed, while an organism in a satiated state (abolishes the effectiveness of food as a reinforcer) is less likely to engage in such behavior.
- Discriminative stimulus (SD) which is a setting or cue, or might be said to be an occasion for a response.
- Behavior is a response (R), typically controlled by past consequences, which is also controlled by the presence of a discriminative stimulus. It operates on the environment, as in having an effect.
- Consequences can consist of reinforcing stimuli (SR) or punishing stimuli (SP) which follow and modify an operant response. Reinforcing stimuli are often classified as positively (Sr+) or negatively reinforcing (Sr−). More complex schedules of reinforcement can also be used.
Anti-theoretical analysis
The idea that Skinner's position is anti-theoretical is probably inspired by the arguments he put forth in his article Are Theories of Learning Necessary?[6] However, this article does not argue against the use of theory as such, only against certain theories in certain contexts. Skinner argued that many theories did not explain behavior, but simply offered another layer of structure that itself had to be explained in turn. If an organism is said to have a drive, which causes its behavior, what then causes the drive? Skinner argued that many theories had the effect of halting research or generating useless research.
Skinner's work did have a basis in theory, though his theories were different from those that he criticized. Mecca Chiesa notes that Skinner's theories are inductively derived, while those that he attacked were deductively derived. The theories that Skinner opposed often relied on mediating mechanisms and structures—such as a mechanism for memory as a part of the mind—which were not measurable or observable. Skinner's theories form the basis for two of his books: Verbal Behavior, and Science and Human Behavior. These two texts represent considerable theoretical extensions of his basic laboratory work into the realms of political science, linguistics, sociology and others.
Notable figures
- Richard Herrnstein – developed the matching law, a mathematical model for decision making, co-authored the controversial The Bell Curve.
- James Holland – co-wrote the highly cited and well-known Principles of Behavior with B.F. Skinner.
- Fred S. Keller – creator of the Personalized System of Instruction (PSI).
- Ogden Lindsley – founder of the Precision Teaching approach to teaching.
- Jack Michael – noted verbal behavior and motivating operations theorist and researcher.
- John Anthony (Tony) Nevin – development behavioral momentum
- Howard Rachlin – pioneer in self-control research and behavioral economics.
- Murray Sidman – discovered Sidman Avoidance, highly cited author, researcher on punishment, also has been influential in research on stimulus equivalence.
- Philip Hineline – contributed extensively to negative reinforcement (escape/avoidance), molecular/molar accounts of behavior processes, and the characteristics of interpretive language.
- Allen Neuringer – well known for theoretical work including volition perception, randomness, self-experimentation, and other areas.
References
- ↑ Chiesa, Mecca: Radical Behaviorism: The Philosophy and the Science (2005)
- ↑ Skinner, B.F.: Are Theories of Learning Necessary? (1951)
- ↑ Skinner, B.F.: The Evolution of Behavior (1984)
- ↑ Catania, A.C. "Learning" Prentice-Hall 1992
- ↑ Laraway, S.; Snycerski, S.; Michael, J.; Poling, A. (2003). "Motivating operations and terms to describe them: some further refinements". J Appl Behav Anal 36 (3): 407–14. doi:10.1901/jaba.2003.36-407. PMC 1284457. PMID 14596584.
- ↑ Skinner, B.F. (July 1950). "Are theories of learning necessary?". Psychol Rev 57 (4): 193–216. doi:10.1037/h0054367. PMID 15440996.
External links
- The Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior has been the flagship journal for behavioral research since 1958 (as a quarterly and since 1964 as a bimonthly publication).
- The Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis explores what is considered to be the more applied areas of the experimental analysis of behavior.
- Behavioural Pharmacology publishes research on the effects of drugs, chemicals, and hormones on schedule-controlled operant behavior, as well as research into "the neurochemical mechanisms underlying behaviour."
- Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin is an online journal publishing experimental research focused on human subjects.
- The Analysis of Verbal Behavior – annual journal for publication of verbal behavior research.
- Are Theories of Learning Necessary? B.F. Skinner's seminal 1950 classic in which he attacks the hypothetico-deductive model of research driven by hypothesis testing.
- Behavioural Processes publishes an annual issue on quantitative analysis of behavior and an issue on Comparative Cognition.