Lengo language
Lengo | |
---|---|
Native to | Solomon Islands |
Region | Guadalcanal |
Native speakers | 14,000 (1999)[1] |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 |
lgr |
Glottolog |
leng1259 [2] |
Lengo is a Southeast Solomonic language of Guadalcanal.
Phonology
Vowels
Lengo has 6 vowels (Unger, 2008:5).
Front | Near-Back | Back | |
---|---|---|---|
Close | i | u | |
Close-Mid | e | o | |
Open-Mid | ɛ | ||
Open | ɑ |
Vowel sequences occur commonly for nearly all combinations of these vowels, with the exceptions of /ɑe/ and /uo/. The front open-mid vowel /ɛ/ never occurs in sequence.
Consonants
Lengo has 15 consonants (Unger, 2008:4).
Labial | Coronal | Velar | |
---|---|---|---|
Nasal | m | n | ŋ |
(prenasalized) Plosive |
ᵐb | ⁿd | ᵑɡ |
p | t | k | |
Spirant | v | ð | ɣ |
Sibilant | s | ||
Trill | r | ||
Lateral | l |
Voiced stops are prenasalized. Two instances of regional variation in these phonemes have been observed. These are /v/ becoming /β/, and /ð/ becoming /z/.
Morphology
Pronominal Systems
Lengo has five sets of pronominal forms. These are emphatic, subject reference, object, direct possessor, and indirect possessor. These distinguish maximally between four persons (first person inclusive and exclusive, second, and third person), and four numbers (singular, plural, dual, and paucal). There is no grammatical gender distinction, but there is an animacy distinction in the object paradigm. Two further uses of these pronominal forms occur - a reflexive pronoun, and a set of interrogative pronouns (Unger, 2008:27-29).
The dual and paucal forms are derived from the plural forms by the addition of ko- and tu- respectively. The dual forms are used only to indicate 'two and only two', whilst the plural and paucal forms mean 'two or more' and 'three or more' respectively. First person exclusive excludes the addresse(s).
Emphatic Pronouns
The emphatic pronoun in Lengo is optional, and can occur in combination with obligatory pronouns that may occur with subject or object function. It can also appear without other pronouns. It is used to emphasize the semantic role of a noun in a clause (Unger, 2008:29).
1st Inclusive | 1st Exclusive | 2nd | 3rd | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Singular | inau | ighoe | igeia | |
Plural | ighita | ighami | ighami | igeira |
Dual | i-ko-ghita | i-ko-ghami | i-ko-ghamu | i-ko-ira |
Paucal | i-tu-ghita | i-tu-ghami | i-tu-ghamu | i-tu-ira |
(Unger, 2008:29)
Examples:
- {|
| (1) ||ara|| gito-a|| t-i|| m-ara|| lavi|| dea-a|| na|| kei-gu|| inau
|-
| ||3PL ||steal-o:3SG ||RL-LOC ||CONJ-3PL ||take ||go-o:3SG ||ART ||basket-PS:1SG ||EP:1SG ||
|-
| ||colspan="9"| "They stole it and they took it away my basket - mine."
|}(Unger, 2008:32)
- {|
| (2) ||ara-ko|| gara|| iti-a|| na|| thinaghe|| i-ko-ira|| m-u|| ghe|| tapa||inau |- | ||3PL-DU ||pull ||up-o:3SG ||ART ||canoe ||DU.EP:3PL ||CONJ-1SG ||continue ||run ||EP:1SG |- | ||colspan="9"| "they two pulled up the canoe and I continued to run." |}(Unger, 2008:30)
Subject Reference Pronouns
The subject reference pronoun appears as the first element in a verb phrase. It is obligatory in any main clause, but can be excepted in subordinate clauses. It is optional in imperative sentences (Unger, 2008:34).
1st Inclusive | 1st Exclusive | 2nd | 3rd | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Singular | u | o | e | |
Plural | a | ami | amu | ara |
Dual | a-ko | ami-ko | amu-ko | ara-ko |
Paucal | a-tu | ami-tu | amu-tu | ara-tu |
(Unger, 2008:37)
Example:
- {|
| (3) || i-ko-ghami|| a P.|| ami-ko|| dea|| i|| nughu. |- | ||DU:EP:1EX.PL ||ART P. ||1EX.PL-DU ||go ||LOC ||river |- | ||colspan="6"|"We two, P. and I, we two went to the river." |} (Unger, 2008:37)
Object Pronouns
The object form in Lengo is identified using a set of pronominal suffixes, which index the object arguments on the verb. In instances where a verb takes both a direct and indirect object, only the indirect object is marked. The third person plural object form is marked for animate or inanimate objects (Unger, 2008:39).
1st Inclusive | 1st Exclusive | 2nd | 3rd | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Singular | -u | -gho | -a | |
Plural | -ghita | -ghami | -ghamu | -ra (animate), -i (inanimate) |
Dual | -ko-ghita | -ko-ghami | -ko-ghamu | -ko-ira |
Paucal | -tu-ghita | -tu-ghami | -tu-ghamu | -tu-ira |
(Unger, 2008:39)
Example:
- {|
| (4) || ara|| pitu-u |- | ||3PL ||wait-o:1SG |- | ||colspan="2"| "They await me." |} (Unger, 2008:39)
Direct Possessor Pronouns
The direct possessor form is used for inalienably possessed nouns. It is a suffix on the possessed noun that indicates the possessor. In the case of the dual and paucal forms, number is indicated as a prefix on the noun, and the plural form of the possessive suffix is used (Unger, 2008:42).
1st Inclusive | 1st Exclusive | 2nd | 3rd | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Singular | -gu | -mu | -a, -na | |
Plural | -da | -mami | -mu | -dira |
Dual | ko- -da | ko- -mami | ko- -miu | ko- -dira |
Paucal | tu- -da | tu- -mami | tu- -miu | tu- -dira |
(Unger, 2008:41)
The third person singular direct possessor appears in two forms, with '-a' being more prevalent than '-na' (Unger, 2008:41).
Examples:
- {|
| (5) || na|| vae-gu
|-
| ||ART ||house-PS:1SG
|-
| ||colspan="2"| "My house."
|} (Unger, 2008:42)
- {|
| (6) || A|| ko-dae-mami ||e ||belo |- | ||ART ||DU-child-PL:1EX.PL ||3SG ||ring.bell |- | ||colspan="4"| "our two's child is ringing the bell." |} (Unger, 2008:49)
Indirect Possessor Pronouns
The indirect possessor form is used for alienably possessed nouns. It occurs as a free morpheme preceding the possessed noun. There are two categories distinguished - 'oral consumable' and 'general'. The oral consumable category includes items that are able to be eaten, drunk, or consumed via the mouth, such as tobacco (Unger, 2008:42).
General | Oral Consumable | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1st Inclusive | 1st Exclusive | 2nd | 3rd | 1st Inclusive | 1st Exclusive | 2nd | 3rd | |
Singular | ni-gu-a | ni-mo-a | ne | gha-gu-a | gha-mo-a | ghe | ||
Plural | no-da | ni-mami | ni-miu | no-dira | gha-da | gha-mami | gha-miu | gha-dira |
Dual | ko-no-da | ko-ni-mami | ko-ni-miu | ko-no-dira | ko-gha-da | ko-gha-mami | ko-gha-miu | ko-gha-dira |
Paucal | tu-no-da | tu-ni-mami | tu-ni-miu | tu-no-dira | tu-gha-da | tu-gha-mami | tu-gha-miu | tu-gha-dira |
(Unger, 2008:42)
Oral Consumable form:
- {|
| (7) || gha-mu-a|| na|| vudi|| lepa
|-
| ||oral.CLF-PS:2SG-O:3SG ||ART ||banana ||ripe
|-
| ||colspan="4"| "[Here is a] ripe banana for you to eat."
|} (Unger, 2008:44)
General form:
- {|
| (8) || ne|| na|| be|| O. |- | ||PS:3SG ||ART ||pig ||O |- | ||colspan="4"| "O's pig." |} (Unger, 2008:43)
Reflexive Pronouns
A reflexive pronoun is composed when a direct possessor suffix is added to the stem 'tibo'. This results in a valency decrease of the verb (Unger, 2008:44).
Examples:
- {|
| (9) || u toka || tibo-gu. ||
|-
| ||1SG cut ||REFL-PS:1SG ||
|-
| ||colspan="2"|"I cut myself."
|} (Unger, 2008:45)
(9) u toka na ghai 1SG cut ART tree "I cut the tree."
Interrogative and Relative Pronouns
Lengo has two pronouns that have interrogative or relative uses. 'thi' is used if the reference is human, and 'tha' if the reference is non-human (Unger, 2008:45).
Relative use:
- {|
| (8) || na || tinoni || ketha || a thi || ga || deni || ba || k-e || mai || lau-a || pile-a || na || vanga || de
|-
| ||ART||person||different||ART REL||there||DEM||FUT||IRR-3SG||come||take-o:3SG||little.bit-o:3SG||ART||food||DEM||
|-
| ||colspan="10"| "a different person who is over there will come take a bit of this food."
|} (Unger, 2008:45)
Interrogative use:
- {|
| (9) || na || tha || t-o || goni-a? |- | ||ART||INT||RL-3SG||do-o:3SG |- | ||colspan="4"|"What are you doing?" |} (Unger, 2008:46)
References
Unger, Paul (2008). Aspects of Lengo Grammar, Canada Institute of Linguistics at Trinity Western University.
|